User Tools

Site Tools



The Theory of Violence

On the violence inhibition mechanism or why many people have a strong inner resistance to harming other people, but there are also those who are able to easily commit a violent attack or even murder, and how to solve this problem

brain.png


When it comes to violence, although it seems to be a socially unacceptable phenomenon, its naturalness is most often not questioned, especially considering the fact that we can observe violence in the animal world and human society. However, if we go deeper into the study of this phenomenon, we can easily realize that everything is not so unambiguous, especially in the case of intraspecific interactions.

Did you know that in the nervous system of many animals, and even humans, there is a mechanism that, when activated, inhibits offensive aggression towards members of their species while not affecting defensive behavior and other forms of activity? And we are talking about intraspecific, not just intragroup social interactions. In particular, this result was demonstrated by experiments with mice and rats. Activation of such a mechanism led to a sharp decrease in their aggressiveness while leaving them with the ability to self-defense and not disrupting their non-aggressive behaviors. Furthermore, a lot of evidence, including the findings of anthropologists and military experts, suggests that the average and healthy individual has a strong inner resistance to killing other people.

The theory that many species have inhibitions of intraspecific aggression, which developed in the course of biological evolution, has existed for quite a long time and originates from the very emergence of ethology, the science of animal behavior. Based on this theory, a model of the violence inhibition mechanism in humans was also proposed, explaining the development of empathy and the emergence of psychopathy. And the results of research in the fields of neurophysiology and genetics confirm its validity.

In turn, the prevalence of violence we observe can be easily explained by the fact that it draws too much attention to itself. One person who committed murder will obviously be more noticeable than a thousand other, peaceful people. Violence is not so common, and it should be considered a deviation and pathology rather than a natural phenomenon of social communication. We will get acquainted with all the details and evidence behind such a conclusion. Also, based on them, we will develop potential solutions to the problem of still-existing violence in society and human relationships.

inhibitor_en.png

▶ The main discussion of the website (free discussion on the problem of violence in human society)


I. Definitions of the concepts of aggression, violence, and self-defense

To make it possible to study the topic of violence as a form of behavior and social communication, we need to give this concept a concrete definition. Not only that, this definition cannot be purely subjective and evaluative; it must be based on concrete, objective data about the nature of the behavior of living beings: animals and humans. And now we will familiarize ourselves with two approaches that will help us with this.


1. Ethological approach

In defining the concept of violence as well as another important concept of self-defense, we will start with the broader concept of aggression. An ethological approach will help us solve this problem. Of course, the definitions thus obtained will differ from many other, more generally accepted definitions. However, exactly these definitions will make it possible for the most objective and accurate distinction between different forms of behavior from a biological point of view, which is extremely important for our study.

To be clear, we should first understand what aggressiveness is. It is a feeling of hostility that arouses thoughts of attack, or simply a natural disposition to be hostile[1]. In turn, aggression is a disposition to behave aggressively, hostilely, unfriendly[2].

However, aggression as a form of behavior and social communication in intraspecific relationships is characterized by constrained actions, reactions, and social signals between participants in the conflict. It is important to pay attention to this “constraint”. It consists in rules and rituals of certain magnitude, expression, and sequence, which make aggression functional, dynamic, yet structured behavior within inhibitory limits. Regardless of species-specific rules, these components are necessary for functionally driven aggression[3][4][5]. Also, this inhibition of aggression is the main function of the violence inhibition mechanism, which we will discuss later.

The difference between violence and functional (or adaptive) aggression lies in the behavioral sequence or interaction dynamics between two or more conspecifics in combat. Violence is characterized by the absence of inhibitory control and the loss of adaptive functions in social communication. As a quantitative behavior, violence is an escalated, pathological, and abnormal form of aggression characterized primarily by short attack latencies, and prolonged and frequent harm-oriented conflict behaviors. As a qualitative behavior, violence is characterized by attacks that are aimed at vulnerable parts of the opponent's body and context-independent attacks regardless of the environment or the sex and type of the opponent[3][4][5][6][7][8][9].

It is believed that functional aggression, unlike violence, is not anticipated to target vulnerable body parts even in the midst of an agonistic interaction unless challenged, as seen in defensive aggression[4][10].

According to the threat superiority effect, humans (like many species) have the ability to quickly and effectively detect threats in the environment, which allows them to activate defense mechanisms in time and adequately respond to the threat[11]. Such a reaction can be expressed by flight or defensive aggression (it is also called a fight-or-flight response). Threat stimuli can be innate due to the fact that humans have encountered them in the course of biological evolution (for example, snakes), or acquired through experience due to the adaptation of defense mechanisms (for example, a knife or a gun)[12][13]. In addition, humans are more likely to recognize angry facial expressions from other humans than neutral and happy expressions, as well as expressions of sadness and fear[14][15].

Self-defense can be defined as a form of aggression performed in the presence of a threat in the environment and social signals[Author's note]. Also, in the case of intraspecific relationships, self-defense (or defensive aggression) is defined as a form of aggressive behavior performed in response to an attack by another individual. It is worth noting that extreme forms of defensive aggression can have violent characteristics. However, it is distinctly different from offense in terms of its behavioral expression and inhibitory control[16][17].

ethological_en.png


2. Reactive and proactive aggression

In studies of the human psyche and behavior, the division of aggression into reactive (affective) and proactive (instrumental) forms is very common. Reactive aggression is an impulsive response to a perceived threat or provocation, associated with high emotional arousal, anxiety, and anger. In turn, proactive aggression is instrumental, organized, cold-blooded, and motivated by the anticipation of reward[18][19].

In other words, reactive aggression arises as a reaction of the subject to a certain stimulus (including a threat stimulus that can lead to self-defense) or as a result of frustration. It is limited to a specific conflict, has no intent, and no purpose other than the direct infliction of harm. And proactive aggression consists in achieving a certain positive result by resorting to aggressive actions; it is a planned and motivated act of harming the victim.

Such a division of the forms of aggression is essential due to the difference in their regulation at the neurophysiological level as well as in the influence of genetics and environment on their manifestation. As for the study of the violence inhibition mechanism, first of all, attention will be focused on proactive, instrumental, and offensive aggression, for the emergence of which its dysfunction is a key component. However, the topic of reactive, impulsive, and affective aggression will not be bypassed either.

proactreact_en.png


II. Myths about violence

In this chapter, we will look at various myths about violence that prevent a full understanding of the nature of this phenomenon. As ethological, archaeological, anthropological, military, and other evidence demonstrates, violence, especially lethal one, is largely absent from intraspecific animal and human relationships. The average and healthy individual has a strong inner resistance to killing, but the minority of killers is still enough for violence to have a huge impact on society and lead to numerous victims.

If you have no doubts about these facts and you are interested in studying the theory of the violence inhibition mechanism directly, then you can immediately skip to Chapter Three.


1. Are intraspecific killings common in mammals and other animals

A study of 1024 mammalian species showed that only about 40% of them were observed to have at least occasional lethal violence – cases of death of individuals from aggressive actions by members of their species (including infanticide, cannibalism, and intergroup aggression). Of course, this figure may be underestimated due to the lack of data, but even after adjusting for this probability, non-violent intraspecific relationships are still common and prevail over violent ones, especially if we take into account that, according to overall statistics, lethal violence is the cause of death in mammals in only 0.3% of cases[20].

Many researchers have come to the conclusion that most intraspecific aggression is non-lethal, and individuals with techniques which enable them to avoid agonistic situations involving serious possibilities of defeat or injury are evolutionarily successful. And restraints against harming and killing conspecifics are common in species with strong innate weapons and social behavior[21][22][23][24][25][26][27][28][29][30]. A detailed description of examples of such restraints and why they have developed during biological evolution can be found in Chapter Three.

As for the relationship of territoriality and social behavior with lethal violence, in both cases there was an elevated level of it, which is easily explained by the increase in the number of conflicts due to more frequent social contacts and territorial fights. However, even for social territorial species, the overall level of lethal violence is 0.8%. Of course, against the background of other mammals, primates stand out with an increased level of violence. However, even in their case, the overall level of lethal violence is 2.3%. And the closest human relative, the pygmy chimpanzee (bonobo), is widely known for its non-violent nature[31].

The highest levels of lethal violence are observed in lemurs, marmosets, and suricates, in which case they can reach almost 20%. However, such cases are an extreme exception, which means we can safely assume that across many species, nonkilling is the default and killing is the exception, the oddity, the unusual[29].

nonkilling_en.png


2. Lethal violence in human history or "The Myth of the Violent Savage"

It is an extremely common claim, taken from the writings of scientist Steven Pinker, that among hunter-gatherer tribes of the past, 15% of people died from lethal violence, and in some cases, its level could be as high as 60%. Thus, societies that existed before the emergence of agricultural civilizations with cities and monopoly governments suffered from chronic violence and endless wars[32][33].

However, arguments of this kind are highly exaggerated and are based on limited examples of societies, not on their overall statistics. A study examining 600 human populations shows that in the entire history of Homo sapiens, the level of lethal violence was only 2%, and this includes cases of war and genocide[20]. Some studies also argue that the theory of the universality of war in human history lacks empirical support, and evidence for high levels of prehistoric violence (such as that demonstrated in the book “War Before Civilization: The Myth of the Peaceful Savage”) may be unreasonably inflated[34][35].

As anthropologist Brian Ferguson writes, considering all the archaeological evidence for Europe and the Near East, and not just selected cases of violence, one can conclude that the idea that 15% of the prehistoric population died from war is not just false, it is absurd. And there is no evidence that war is an expression of innate human tendencies or a selective force driving human psychological evolution[36].

Pinker ignored much of the archaeological evidence that did not correlate with his argument. One survey of 2000–3000 remains found in France showed 1.9% with projectile wounds, including healed ones. One site in Britain of 350 individuals showed about 2% with identifiable trauma. Another site in Serbia and Romania of 418 individuals showed 2.3% with signs of violent injury. A study of 2500 remains of adults found in Japan showed that 2% died potentially violently. Anthropologist Radovanovic, after studying 1107 remains from Europe, including all the cases on Pinker's list, concludes that, on average, you can get a 3.7% level of lethal violence as a low estimate and 5.5% as a high estimate. These results are not even close to Pinker's 15%[36][37][38].

Claims about extremely high levels of violence in prehistoric men are often based on the analogy with the high levels of violence in some modern hunter-gatherer tribes. However, a study of 21 nomadic tribal societies shows that in 10 of them only one person committed killings, and in 3 of them there was no killing at all. Nearly half of the killings (47%) occurred in the Tiwi tribe from Australia, which shows its exceptional propensity for violence. Also, anthropologist Douglas Fry, after studying the anthropological literature, found as many as 70 nonwarring cultures, including cases of completely non-violent tribes, famous examples of which are the Paliyar (or Paliyan) from South India and the Semai from Malaysia[39][40][38]. Although homicide rates vary tremendously from one society to the next and also change over time within the same society, the vast majority of people never kill or attempt to kill anyone[30].

What also turned out to be false was the claim made by anthropologist Napoleon Chagnon, who studied the Yanomami tribes and is often cited by Pinker, that in tribal societies, men who commit killings should be more reproductively successful (have 3 times as many children) as they eliminate their neighbors from procreation. And since in the past all people lived in tribes, this allegedly made a human a natural-born killer. But the studies that make such a claim have methodological errors, the difference in average age between the killers and non-killers studied is more than 10 years, which distorts the results. And even if they were the same age (Chagnon insists that they were, but flatly refuses to provide evidence of this), other anthropologists' calculations suggest that such results would still be exaggerated. Also, they do not agree with the findings of many other studies, which show that killers not only have the same number of children as non-killers but also that the children of killers are less likely to reach reproductive age[30][41][42].

It is worth briefly mentioning the issue of cannibalism. The problem with it arises from the fact that often researchers mistake cases of ritual consumption of dead relatives (endocannibalism) for cases of consumption of enemies defeated in a war (exocannibalism)[43][36].

In general, it is incorrect to describe human violence based only on individual cases of extremely violent tribes. And in addition, based on cases of violence in modern tribes, one should not lightly draw conclusions about the level of lethal violence in the tribes of the past – a mistake that some researchers make[34][30].

ferguson_en.png


3. War and resistance to killing

Military experts have found that most humans possess an intense resistance to killing. The resistance is so strong that, in many circumstances, soldiers on the battlefield will die before they can overcome it. Overall, a strong inhibitor to killing is inherent in 98% of soldiers. This means that there is only 2% of the male population that, if pushed or if given a legitimate reason, will kill without regret or remorse[44][45][46][30]. And we will now familiarize ourselves with the range of evidence behind such a conclusion. Also, along the way, we will examine the criticisms it faces and demonstrate their untenable points.

A study by psychologists Roy Swank and William Marchand, published after World War II, demonstrated that after 60 days of ongoing battles, 98% of surviving soldiers are psychologically traumatized, and only less than 2% of them who are predisposed to be “aggressive psychopaths” are not concerned with this kind of problem since they apparently do not experience any resistance to killing[47][44][45]. And according to the American military commander, brigadier general, and historian Samuel Marshall, among American soldiers, only 15–20% fired at enemy positions during World War II. In many cases, those who did not fire were willing to risk great danger to rescue comrades, get ammunition, or run messages. And studies by army psychiatrists show that the biggest cause of combat defeats in the European theater of World War II was the fear of killing other people and not, as many might think, the fear of being killed (or wounded), which came in second place[44][48].

It is worth noting that Marshall's conclusions are sometimes criticized[49]. However, as American publicist and former Lieutenant Colonel Dave Grossman writes, the US Armed Forces have widely accepted them. Although Marshall's methodology may not meet rigorous modern standards, that does not mean he lied, and every available, parallel, scholarly study validates his basic findings. In support of his words, Grossman cites such war researchers as Ardant du Picq, John Keegan, Richard Holmes, and Paddy Griffith. The evidence they and many other researchers provide is compiled in his book “On Killing: The Psychological Cost of Learning to Kill in War and Society”, which is on the curriculum of many American institutions and the U.S. Marine Corps Commandant's Reading List. According to Grossman, in the realms of criminal justice, psychology, sociology, and peace studies programs, the possible existence of an innate resistance to killing, in most healthy citizens, is widely accepted[44][50].

There is some criticism towards Grossman as well. For example, his idea that video games make people more violent and train them to be killers has been shown to be unfounded. And his promotion of military training for police officers, including training them to kill, has been criticized for the fact that it could lead to more police violence against ordinary citizens[51][52][53][54]. But none of this is relevant to the current topic. Grossman may be a controversial person who is wrong about some things, but his position on the existence of resistance to killing is well-founded[Author's note].

The only thing worth mentioning, apart from what has already been said, is the criticism from anthropologist Michael Ghiglieri. He is a proponent of the idea that humans have an instinct to commit murder, rape, and genocide, developed over millions of years of evolution. And those who argue otherwise, including Grossman, in his opinion, simply do not understand biology. But in a review of his book “The Dark Side of Man: Tracing the Origins of Male Violence”, anthropologist Brian Ferguson writes that it is full of arguments by analogy, sweeping generalizations, and one-sided presentations. It also puts forward major misinformation that is inconsistent with the literature on the topic of violence, according to which the decision to kill in men is triggered by just one chemical, testosterone. However, Ferguson praises Ghiglieri for the fact that his book was skillfully written to convince people who are already primed to believe men are bad to the bone[55][56]. Now, having dealt with the critique, we can continue to explore the topic of resistance to killing.

Back in the middle of the 19th century, French army officer and military theorist Charles Ardant du Picq conducted his own research – a survey among other officers, who told him that many soldiers simply shoot in the air without aiming[57]. And the book “Soldiers” by John Keegan and Richard Holmes provides one interesting example. At the beginning of the 18th century, at the Battle of Belgrade, there was a case when two imperial battalions fired at Turkish enemies until they approached them at a distance of only 30 paces. However, as a result, they were able to kill only 32 Turkish soldiers. At the same time, an experiment conducted a little earlier in the Prussian army showed that soldiers hit non-living targets from a distance of 225 yards (205 meters) in 25% of cases, and from a distance of 75 yards (68 meters) – in 60% of cases[58]. With a potential hit rate of well over 50% at the average combat ranges of that era, the killing rate should have been hundreds per minute, instead of one or two. The weak link between the killing potential and the killing capability was the soldier who, when faced with a living opponent instead of a target, simply fired over his head[44]. Military theorist and historian Paddy Griffith makes the same conclusion, arguing that only a small percentage of the musketeers were actually attempting to shoot at the enemy while the rest stood bravely in line firing above the enemy's heads or did not fire at all[59].

Grossman notes that researchers such as Marshall, Keegan, Holmes, and Griffith provide many cases where soldiers, when confronted face-to-face on the battlefield, simply refused to shoot each other and split apart. Looking another person in the face, seeing their eyes and fear, being in a situation where it is necessary to kill not a generalized enemy but a specific individual, it becomes extremely difficult to deny their humanity[44].

The Battle of Gettysburg, the bloodiest battle of the American Civil War, is quite a demonstrative example. Of the approximately 170,000 soldiers involved, approximately 7,000 died, and after the battle, more than 27,000 abandoned muskets were found, 90% of which were loaded, and 12,000 muskets were loaded multiple times. As Canadian historian, journalist, and retired naval officer Gwyn Dyer writes, this could mean that most of the soldiers on both sides were loading their muskets, perhaps even pretending to shoot if someone nearby actually fired, but couldn't fire themselves. And many of those who did shoot most likely did not aim at the enemy[60][45]. Of course, some might say that the soldiers simply made mistakes in the use of weapons. But even if, despite all the endless hours of training, you do accidentally double-load a musket, you shoot it anyway, and the first load simply pushes out the second load. And in the rare event of a weapon breaking, you can pick up another one. But that's not what actually happened, and it is doubtful that 12,000 men from both sides made the exact same mistake[44].

Dyer also cites one interesting fact from the statistics of the US Air Force. Less than 1% of pilots accounted for about 40% of downed enemy aircraft. Most of the pilots did not shoot down anyone and did not even try to shoot down. In addition, when the US Air Force tried to identify commonalities among their World War II aces, it was found that they had been involved in a lot of fights as children. And they were not just bullies who, as a rule, avoided real fights, they were exactly “fighters”[60][44].

Looking back at how many victims some wars, and especially World War II, had, it is difficult to agree that only 2% of soldiers actually killed their enemies. However, this can be explained by distancing, which allows soldiers not to perceive their potential victims as real and concrete people. Dyer notes that strong resistance to killing was not observed in artillerymen, bomber crew members, naval personnel, and machine gunners, who, without seeing their target, were able to convince themselves that they did not kill anyone at all[60][44].

It should also be noted that soldier training after World War II began to take into account the existence of resistance to killing. They were made more effective, and the number of soldiers shooting in combat increased a lot (although this still doesn't tell us anything about how many of them actually aim at the enemy). However, soldiers who do find themselves capable of killing after such training are later unable to cope with what they have done and begin to suffer serious psychological trauma. The cost in the form of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) after killing is, in most cases, unavoidable. It is pertinent to recall the words of military historian Richard Gabriel, who has studied the issue of psychological trauma in soldiers, that the cost of war is usually measured in dollars, lost production, or the number of soldiers killed or wounded, but rarely in terms of individual suffering[44][46][61].

The traumatic impact of war on the human psyche is also confirmed by the prevalence of suicide among veterans[46][61]. An illustrative example is that since 2001, about 7,000 soldiers have died in US military operations in Iraq and Afghanistan. At the same time, by 2021, about 30,000 veterans committed suicide. Participation in military operations increases the risk of suicide by 50%[62].

At the end, it is worth noting that there is a statement that roughly 80% of males choose to avoid violent conflict. If forced into violent conflict, they just do not fight, although present. The 20% left does not reject violence as a behavioral option. Nevertheless, the main part is probably defensive only, that is, they use violence only if compelled to. Finally, about 1% adopts an offensive elementary strategy. Historical and statistical facts confirm the existence of a ratio noncombatants : defensive combatants : offensive combatants. Roughly, this ratio looks like 80:19:1[45]. This statement is mentioned by researcher Johan M. G. van der Dennen, who has also done a good job collecting evidence on resistance to killing. However, its primary source is an “unpublished manuscript” that cannot be found, so we will leave it to your judgment[Author's note].

war_cite_en.png

▶ See also: Documentary film "The Truth About Killing" (2004)


4. How many people participate in committing genocides

We can look at the horror of the Pol Pot regime, which led to the deaths of almost a quarter of Cambodian population. Or we can look at the genocide of the Tutsi people in Rwanda, which is usually blamed on the entire Hutu people, using terms such as “criminal population”. However, in assessing such phenomena as war or genocide, one should definitely not rely on subjective feelings, where it would be better to carry out detailed calculations.

It is known that the Khmer Rouge exterminated about 1.8 million Cambodians between 1975 and 1979. Khmer Rouge forces in 1975 consisted of 55 to 70 thousand people, in 1976 – 72 thousand people, and in 1979 – 80 thousand people. And the population of Cambodia was about 7.3 to 7.9 million at the beginning of the genocide[63][64][65][66][67][68]. If we take the ratio of Khmer Rouge to Cambodian population aged 15 to 64 (it was 55% of the total population), we get an estimate of less than 2% of the genocide perpetrators[Author's note].

Based on the most widely accepted studies, between 500,000 and 662,000 Tutsis died as a result of the genocide in Rwanda, some estimates reach up to 800,000 dead[69][70]. So how many Hutus took part in the genocide? One study suggests that the number of murderers should be 50,000. It also states that the genocide was not a spontaneous eruption of tribal hatreds, as it was portrayed by the Western media, this was a coordinated attack by a small core with no more than two dozen leaders and no more than 100,000 of their henchmen in the state machinery (including the military)[71]. Another study estimates the number of participants in the genocide (those who committed murder attempts, murder, rape, torture, and other forms of serious violence) from 175,000 to 210,000 people[72]. The maximum estimates of the number of those who committed at least one act of genocidal violence reach 234,000 people[73].

What does this mean? The vast majority of the Hutu people, and even the majority of their active adult (aged 18 to 54) male population, which was about 1.26 million people, did not take any violent part in the genocide. The worst possible measure of such participation we can get is if we take the ratio of the maximum estimate of genocidal violence perpetrators to the active adult male Hutu population – it is almost 19%. While this is an extremely high and extraordinary figure, there is still no question of a “criminal population” and collective guilt. And we should not forget that this is still the worst possible figure, and in fact, some of the violent offenders were not part of the active adult male Hutu population (but estimating their exact number is problematic). If we take the ratio of the maximum estimate of violent offenders to the entire active adult Hutu population, which was about 2.6 million people, we get 9%. And if we take the low estimate (175,000), we get less than 7%[72][Author's note].

It is worth noting some important considerations. The study estimating the number of murderers in the Rwandan genocide at 50,000 people states that it is not impossible that even 25,000 people could kill hundreds of thousands, if not a million civilians in 100 days. Think about it – in order for such a scenario to become a reality, one murderer needs to commit only one murder every two and a half days[71]. There is also evidence that in one of the Rwandan military camps there were 2,000 well-trained soldiers, and of these, just 40 people could kill up to 1,000 Tutsis in 20 minutes[74][75].

It is also worth noting cases where one individual personally killed thousands of people at once. For example, the Croatian war criminal Petar Brzica killed up to 1360 Serbs in one night[76]. And the NKVD officer Vasily Mikhailovich Blokhin shot up to 20,000 people in his entire service[77]. Such cases only confirm the fact that the murderers, in the presence of an unlimited opportunity to murder, will personally commit dozens, hundreds, and possibly thousands of murders. Accordingly, we should always expect that the number of murderers relative to the number of murdered will be quite small[Author's note].


5. What kind of people commit violent crimes and harm others

According to one Swedish study, the 1% of the population is responsible for 63% of all violent crimes. The sample of this study consisted of 2,393,765 individuals over a time period of 32 years. Murder, assault, robbery, threats, a gross violation of an individual's integrity, kidnapping, arson, intimidation, unlawful coercion, and illegal confinement were categorized as violent crimes (including attempts to commit them when applicable). Only 93,462 (i.e., 3.9%) out of all individuals considered committed at least one such crime, among which 21,530 individuals (i.e., 0.9%) committed from 3 to 10 crimes and 2,812 (i.e., 0.1%) more than 10 crimes. In total, they committed 234,383 violent crimes, or 2.5 crimes per one criminal[78].

Crime, including violent crime, does not have a universal and biologically determined relationship with age. Although in practice the main share of crimes is committed at the age of 15–25, after which criminal activity decreases, this is more explained not by biological but by social factors (creating a family, finding a legal income, etc.)[79]. However, the vast majority of people never commit premeditated violent attacks anyway, especially those that cause serious harm. But who is the violent minority? These are people with psychopathic predispositions!

In one study, 98 forensic men charged with violent crimes showed an average PCL-R score of 21.4 out of 40. And only 9 individuals (9.2%) had a psychopathy score below 10[80]. A similar Swedish study of 43 men, 74.4% of whom were charged with violent crimes, showed an average psychopathy score of 23.08[81]. For murderers on death row in California, the average psychopathy score was 23.31. Only 15% of the individuals had a score of 10 or less. Some of these offenders had no official criminal history prior to their capital crimes, were contrite, apologetic, and remorseful during their court proceedings, and generally engaged in normative conduct for the majority of their adult lives. These are people who most would view as “salvageable.” Individuals with higher psychopathy scores fared more poorly. And among the five people who scored a maximum of 40 points were the most violent criminals – serial sexual murderers[82].

Also, one study provides a meta-analysis of 22 studies with 29 unique samples of homicide offenders from 6 countries: the USA, Canada, Sweden, Finland, Germany, and Brazil. It found that the average murderer's PCL-R score is 21.1 out of 40. At the same time, for people who do not commit crimes, it is only 5.2[83]. In general, the majority of the population is characterized by low levels or complete absence of psychopathic traits and corresponding behaviors. According to non-criminal and non-psychiatric samples, more than 80–90% of people have low levels of psychopathy. And only 1–2% of people have high levels of psychopathy (above 12 out of 24 on the PCL:SV test)[84][85].

Clinical psychopaths, scoring from 25–30 on the PCL-R test and from 18 on the PCL:SV test, make up no more than 1% of people in society. However, among incarcerated criminals, they can be as high as 25%. It has also been found that if people are divided into two equal groups on the basis of their PCL:SV scores, individuals in the higher-scoring group are 10 times more likely to commit violent crimes. Estimates of the economic burden of crime resulting from clinical psychopathy as of 2020 range from $245.5 billion to $1.59 trillion per year (1.1–7.4% of GDP) in the case of the United States and from CAD12.14 to CAD53 billion per year (0.5–2.3% of GDP) in the case of Canada[86][87].

An increased number of psychopathic individuals may show up in some professions, for example, managers and CEOs. According to various studies and claims, between 3% and 21% of people among their representatives are psychopaths[88][89]. Also, one study conducted among employees of companies showed that if a company employs non-psychopathic managers (whose psychopathy scores are less than 9 out of 16 on the PM-MRV test), the overwhelming majority of employees (89.3%) will assess its activities as socially responsible and environmentally friendly. However, this figure drops to 66% in the presence of dysfunctional managers (scored at 9–12 points) and to 52.5% in the presence of psychopathic managers (scored at more than 12 points). In addition, the majority of employees (79.6%) think that companies with non-psychopathic managers show commitment to them, but this figure drops to as low as 23.7% if psychopathic managers are present. In general, it is a widely known fact that psychopathic individuals working in companies are prone to white-collar crime, such as embezzlement and fraud. These results demonstrate the importance of the problem of corporate psychopaths, who may make ethically questionable decisions in pursuit of their own benefit and have a negative impact on their company and society as a whole[90][91].

Politicians cannot be expected to do well either; despite the lack of reliable statistics, practically any expert in the field of sociopathy/psychopathy/antisocial personality disorder would not dispute that there is a higher percentage of psychopathically predisposed individuals among them than in society as a whole[92].

The average PCL-R score among army and police officers convicted of crimes against humanity – mass arrests, tortures, and murders – is 21.06. State violators of human rights have an extreme disposition for self-serving, callous, and ruthless treatment of others, without guilt or remorse[93].

It is worth noting that the more psychopathic an individual is, the more proactive (instrumental) aggressive behavior can be expected in the crimes they commit. At the same time, non-psychopathic individuals are characterized only by reactive (affective) aggression. As research demonstrates, committing just one act of proactive violence is already associated with increased psychopathic predispositions of the offender compared to offenders whose actions were purely reactive[94][95][96]. It is crucial to understand that even among murderers there are non-psychopathic individuals, but this happens due to affective reasons. Whereas in the case of premeditated and cold-blooded violent crimes, we can always expect increased psychopathic predispositions in their perpetrators[Author's note].

psychopathy_en.png


6. What famous experiments say about violence

“Universe 25”

“Universe 25” was a famous experiment in which ethologist John Calhoun created a habitat for mice with an abundance of resources. Initially, the population of mice grew rapidly up to 2200 individuals, but after that, mice began to refuse to reproduce, their number began to decline, and in less than 5 years, the population completely died out. Drawing an analogy to human society, Calhoun concluded that exceeding a certain population density leads to degradation of the behavior of individuals, breakdown of social bonds, and later to complete extinction[97].

This experiment was criticized for making many mistakes, for example, the living conditions of the mice were actually far from ideal. But few people are aware of the fact that the main mistake was the structure of the habitat, which allowed the 65 largest males to forcefully block all others from accessing females and food. This caused a chain of events that led to the extinction of the population. In more well-organized habitats, where it is impossible to establish such a violent dominance hierarchy, a population of mice can live for decades[98]. This experiment demonstrates well why, under certain conditions, violence is a threat to the survival of the population and is not an evolutionarily stable strategy[Author's note].

The Milgram experiment

In 1963, psychologist Stanley Milgram decided to conduct a series of experiments to clarify the question of how much suffering ordinary people are willing to inflict on other, completely innocent people, if it is part of their duties. The subjects, being in the role of “teacher,” had to punish the “learner” with an electric shock in cases of incorrect performance of tasks. Starting at 15 volts, with each new error, they had to increase the shock by 15 volts up to a maximum of 450 volts, after which they had to continue to use the maximum shock. Of course, the learner, being an actor, did not receive a shock and only pretended to be in pain. In different versions of the experiment, the learner and the teacher were separated either by a soundproof wall (i.e., the teacher could only hear the learner knocking on the wall) or by an ordinary one (i.e., the teacher could hear screams, requests to stop, or complaints about alleged problems with the heart).

According to published data, one of the series of experiments showed that 26 subjects out of 40 (65%) increased the voltage up to 450 volts and did not stop delivering electric shocks until the researcher gave the order to end the experiment. And only 5 subjects (12.5%) stopped at 300 volts when their victims showed the first signs of discontent[99]. Reproduction of the experiment in different conditions and with different people, as stated, showed approximately the same results[100].

However, we will see a very different result if we take into account the data from the Milgram experiment that has not been published. After analyzing 656 post-experimental questionnaires, the researchers found that 56% of the participants actually stopped the experiment at one point or another because they believed the person behind the wall was actually in pain. Another study, looking at 91 interviews conducted immediately after the experiments, found that among 46 participants who continued the experiment after the victim was dissatisfied, 33 participants (72%) did so because they simply did not believe that the victim was really in pain (which was actually the case – the actor only imitated it)[101][102][103].

This experiment also has serious methodological problems. As we know, the researchers put strong pressure on the “teachers,” often going beyond the protocol of the experiment. There is also no evidence that all people perceive authority in the same way. And finally, the person who played the “learner” was not a professional actor, the experiment was based on the deception of the subject, and there is reason to believe that unconsciously most people would recognize real pain or its absence[104]. These problems also make any attempt to repeat the Milgram experiment questionable[Author's note].

The Stanford Prison Experiment

Another well-known experiment about violence is the Stanford Prison Experiment. The participants of this experiment were divided into two groups: the guards and the prisoners, who lived in a simulated prison. Soon after the start of the experiment, the guards began to brutally abuse the prisoners, with a third of them showing sadistic tendencies. Two prisoners were even removed from the experiment due to the psychological trauma they received, and the experiment itself was stopped ahead of time for ethical reasons.

For almost 50 years, many believed in the truthfulness of these results. However, the experiment turned out to be completely untenable. The guards were aware of the results that were expected from them and received clear instructions. Potential participants knew in advance what awaited them in the experiment and what roles they would play. And after a while, some of them said that they had just “played” their role. One of the expelled participants later admitted that he was only faking psychosis because he did not like the experiment and wanted to leave as soon as possible. Finally, the data researchers published were far from complete; out of the 150 hours of the experiment, only 10% have been recorded (6 hours of video and 15 hours of audio). Also, very little personal data about the participants was collected, which could affect the course of the experiment[105][106].

Conclusions about the experiments on violence

Such experiments very often create myths around themselves that do not correspond to reality. In the case of “Universe 25,” the method of conducting the experiment and the interpretation of the results were incorrect. Much of the data from the Milgram experiment was simply hidden in the archive. And the Stanford prison experiment turned out to be a staged production with a predetermined result. Therefore, it is always worth questioning each such experiment. It is likely that the most popular interpretation will be fundamentally wrong.

It is worth remembering another experiment – the performance of the artist Marina Abramovic called “Rhythm 0,” in which she completely surrendered to the will of the audience, allowing them to freely use 72 objects and her body. As a result, for 6 hours of the performance, she was brutally tortured and even almost shot. It was concluded that all people are cruel, and under suitable conditions, this cruelty will surely break out.

So far, there are no refutations of this experiment. But it can be assumed that it was either staged, like the Stanford prison experiment, with which it is sometimes compared, or the audience was unrepresentative, or cruel people were specially selected as the audience (in many of her performances, Abramovich deliberately put herself in danger and almost died several times)[Author's note]. At least Abramovich's past performances could determine the audience and its expectations, and the environment she created and the objects she selected set the context of what was happening, quite probably also reflecting the hidden desires of Abramovich, about whom this performance speaks as a manipulative person[104]. Note that such assumptions can be put forward for any experiment that allegedly proves the violent nature and cruelty of a human[Author's note].


7. Violence draws too much attention to itself

Sometimes it is stated that not a single day in human history has passed without violence and military conflicts. So, it should be a natural phenomenon for humans and human society. However, this opinion is based more on the subjective evaluation of events taking place in the world than on real data, as well as on the excessive visibility of violence against the background of all other events.

There is one illustrative example of how violence can attract significant attention: 69% of Americans believe that domestic violence is a common problem among American football players. This belief is based on media scandals unfolding around players who have actually committed violence. But if we rely on statistics and not on subjective feelings, it turns out that in the families of American football players, domestic violence occurs almost 2 times less often than on average in American families. At the same time, there is a serious problem of domestic violence in the families of police officers; in them, it occurs up to 4 times more often than on average. However, this is information that is often not publicized and investigated[107][108][109][110][111][112].

Observing violence makes people believe that it is common. But to give a real assessment, one should rely only on real data and not on arbitrary statements[Author's note].


8. Misconceptions of dystopian literature on the nature of violence

Many dystopian stories create the false impression that a peaceful person, totally incapable of committing violent attacks, must necessarily be a passive and unmotivated individual. Of course, aggressive stimulus can be important for an individual in many activities. But one should not equate functional aggression with violence.

In Stanislaw Lem's “Return from the Stars,” in order to maintain a peaceful society, people are treated with a procedure called “betrization,” designed to neutralize aggressive impulses in the brain and strengthen the self-preservation instinct. But in reality, people do not necessarily need to have no aggressive impulses or strong fear for their lives to be absolutely peaceful and non-psychopathic. They only need to have strong reflexes and emotions that will impose inhibitory limits on aggression, causing them to have an inner resistance to harming other people. Lem appears to have been mistaken in his understanding of the real nature of violence.

Another book called “A Clockwork Orange” by Anthony Burgess is based on the author's view that all human beings have an inner drive to commit violence, provoked by “original sin,” and to take away an individual's freedom to choose whether or not to commit violence is a sinful act. Obviously, a work based on a view that normalizes violence is not something we can take seriously. Many people have a strong inner resistance to committing violence, and they certainly do not look like the protagonist of this work after brainwashing that made him unable to defend himself and listen to his favorite music.

As we can see, representations of fictional works about the nature of violence can be extremely misleading. This is always worth mentioning when someone cites them as an argument[Author's note].


III. The Theory of the Violence Inhibition Mechanism

With plenty of evidence that in many circumstances aggressive behavior is restrained and that normally people have a strong inner resistance to killing, we can proceed to an explanation of this phenomenon. To understand the evolutionary reasons for its emergence, we will first look at the theory of intraspecific aggression inhibitions in animals, after which we will move on to the theory of the violence inhibition mechanism in humans.


1. Evolution of intraspecific aggression inhibitions in animals

In interspecific interactions, the role of aggression is quite obvious, for example, in predation and defense. And it is also important in intraspecific relationships, for example, in the division of territory, in the competition for the possibility to reproduce, as well as in the establishment and maintenance of social hierarchy.

Nevertheless, do not make the mistake of looking at aggression in isolation from external factors. The two most important of these factors are the presence of strong innate weapons in conspecifics and their lack of opportunity to escape from each other (due to a limited area of habitat, social behavior, or other reasons), which leads to frequent conflict situations. The more pronounced these two factors are, the greater the risks aggressive behavior creates. As a result, its unrestrained forms cease to be an evolutionarily stable strategy of behavior as they begin to interfere with survival, and natural selection directs towards the development of strong restraints, preventing the infliction of serious harm and killing between conspecifics.

Observations of animal behavior support this conclusion. The concept of aggression inhibitions was first formulated by the ethologist Konrad Lorenz. According to his theory, they are most developed in those species which members are able to easily (with a single peck or bite) kill an individual of approximately their own size. Describing his own observations of wolves, Lorenz showed how aggression inhibitions are activated when one wolf demonstrates to another a gesture of submission or vulnerable parts of its body, such as the neck or belly. As a result, a petrified aggressor cannot continue the attack. Also, observations of ravens showed that they do not peck out each other's eyes, even during fights[21][22].

The ethologist Irenaus Eibl-Eibesfeldt listed many such observations from various researchers[23]. Fiddler crabs, due to their anatomical features, do not open their claws in fights wide enough to injure an opponent[113][114]. Many species of fish, lizards, and mammals are characterized by the ritualization of fights. A noteworthy example is oryx antelopes, which carefully handle their sharp horns in fights with other oryx but at the same time use them to the full extent in defense against lions[115]. It is also worth mentioning venomous snakes, many of which squirm, bloat, and push each other during fights but do not bite or even display their weapons[23][116]. Even very primitive creatures have a similar mechanism. So, jellyfish have a chemical blocker that prevents stinging a conspecific. At the same time, all other living beings are stung automatically[117].

Aggression is less inhibited in weakly armed species. Compared to ravens, turtledoves with a less sharp beak can even kill a conspecific if it is deprived of the opportunity to escape (for example, when placed in a cage). Under natural conditions, conflicts do not threaten the survival of turtledoves in any way, they are unable to kill a conspecific quickly, and it can easily escape. Animals with a solitary lifestyle are also quite aggressive. For example, conflicts cannot threaten the survival of polar bears or jaguars, which, out of the breeding season, rarely cross each other's paths for the resulting fights to have any effect on the population as a whole[21][22].

Other selection factors may also lead to a decrease in aggressive behavior in a population. For example, there is a quite famous story from biologist Robert Sapolsky, who has been observing baboons for decades. Alpha males in the observed flock behaved quite aggressively towards their kin. But at one point, the flock discovered a garbage pit, which only these alpha males could approach since they had to fight with the alpha males of another flock for it. At one point, they caught an infection in the garbage pit, after which they died, leaving the flock without overly aggressive individuals. And the most interesting begins further – even though the flock continued to have a hierarchy, fights and violence between its members stopped, and this result was preserved for decades when the original males had already died of old age[118]. Also note that in the case of pygmy chimpanzees (bonobos), it is common for females to gather in groups to defend themselves against aggressive males (obviously, this would not favor aggression in natural selection)[31].

We should also not forget such a factor of selection against aggressive behavior as inclusive fitness. The basis of evolution is the preservation and spread of genes. And one and the same gene, carriers of which kill each other, has fewer chances for this. Accordingly, it is evolutionarily beneficial to develop mechanisms that restrain aggression between individuals sharing enough of the same genes. Among other things, inclusive fitness may be one of the evolutionary factors that led to the development of aggression inhibitions in humans, despite the fact that, according to Lorenz, due to weak innate weapons, humans have rather weak aggression inhibitions that do not cover the use of the artificial weapons they have created[21][29][119]. Lorenz was concerned about the consequences of humans becoming the most armed species on the planet. However, due to evolutionary reasons, the vast majority of humans fundamentally cannot be psychopathic individuals; human society is only able to exist if their number is limited[120][121]. The average and healthy individual still has a strong inner resistance to killing, and “it gives us cause to believe that there may just be hope for mankind after all”[44].

It is necessary to take into account that some unknown and still unstudied factors can weaken aggression inhibitions, as it happens, for example, in lions, which are strongly armed and social species, but sometimes kill even members of their own pride. Also, Lorenz's theoretical developments are sometimes criticized, for example, there is criticism of his hydraulic model of aggression, which states that organisms have a tendency to accumulate aggressive “energy” that is later released in the form of aggressive behavior even in the absence of external stimuli to provoke it; among other things, this explains spontaneous acts of aggression. However, Lorenz himself recognized the limitations of this model and that it has a number of shortcomings. In addition, there are studies confirming the existence of such a mechanism. Moreover, the criticism of Lorenz does not concern his theory of aggression inhibitions[22][122][123][Author's note].

Finally, it is necessary to mention the parochial altruism hypothesis. Based on it, aggression is restrained only between members of one group, and intragroup altruism even contributes to the growth of aggression towards members of other, “alien” groups[124][125]. This hypothesis, at first glance, contradicts the theory of aggression inhibitions. However, this is not necessarily the case, as we will see in the sixth topic of Chapter Four.

inhibition_eng.png


2. Self-defense as an evolutionarily stable strategy of behavior

As we discovered earlier, committing violent attacks is not an evolutionarily stable strategy of behavior for species, members of which have strong innate weapons and lack the opportunity to avoid each other. The most aggressive individuals that often initiate attacks will also die more often due to the weapons and resistance of their victims. As a result, there will be evolutionary pressure to develop inhibitions of intraspecific aggression, or so-called violence inhibitor, since individuals lacking such a mechanism will be less likely to pass on their genes further. But it is worth understanding one important point: this will not work if the victim of the attack cannot use its weapons in self-defense. This leads us to the assumption that in the presence of an immediate threat to life, the function of the violence inhibitor should be suppressed for a short period of time, sufficient to fight back against the aggressor[Author's note].

This assumption is consistent with the concept of the threat superiority effect, which we considered at the beginning of our study. According to it, the presence of a threat in the environment and social signals leads to the activation of defense mechanisms and the suppression of other ongoing cognitive processes. In behavior, this effect is often manifested by a fight-or-flight response[11][12][13].

Also, computer simulations of evolutionary processes have shown that in most cases, neither the belligerent strategy (hawk), which consists in making attacks, nor the timid strategy (dove), which consists in retreating when attacked, are not as evolutionarily stable strategies as the retaliator strategy, which means to behave non-aggressively but in the event of an attack to fight back. Timid individuals cannot compete with aggressive individuals, but aggressive individuals risk getting hurt in fights. Therefore, the mixed retaliator strategy is the most stable[126][127][128][30].


3. The Violence Inhibition Mechanism in humans

Neuroscientist James Blair suggested that humans possess aggression inhibitions similar to those observed in many animals in intraspecific relationships, and proposed the Violence Inhibition Mechanism (VIM) model. In developing the VIM model, he also aimed to explain the development of empathy as a result of the functioning of this mechanism and the emergence of psychopathy as a result of its dysfunction[129][130].

VIM is a cognitive mechanism that is directly activated in individuals by the observation of non-verbal distress cues from other individuals, such as a sad facial expression or crying. This causes an aversive reaction, and the stronger the distress cues, the stronger the corresponding reaction: a slight sadness on the face will cause only partial aversion, but screams and sobbing can completely stop the aggressor. Also, VIM is not just a mechanism consisting of an unconditioned reflex (aversive reaction) triggered by an unconditioned stimulus (distress cues). Blair argues that through the process of conditioning (the formation of conditioned reflexes), it becomes a cognitive prerequisite for the development of three aspects of morality: the moral emotions (i.e., sympathy, guilt, remorse, and empathy), the inhibition of violence, and the ability to distinguish between moral and conventional transgressions.

During normal development, individuals will witness other individuals displaying distress cues, resulting in the activation of VIM. On many occasions, the observers may role take with the distressed victims in order to understand their state. In this way, an association of the distress cues that activate VIM with the state of the victim is formed. This association becomes the conditioned stimulus for the conditioned reflex. As a result, the individual becomes able to show an empathic response only by thinking about someone else's distress. In line with this, film sequences where the victims of violence talked about their experience while not showing any distress cues evoked a corresponding physiological reaction in the audience[131][132][133][130].

The inhibition of violence works similarly. As early as childhood (at the age of 4–7 years), normally developing individuals will experience the activation of VIM and an aversive reaction due to the victim's distress cues as soon as they attempt to commit an act of violence (or even take possessions from another child without their permission)[134]. Over time, even the very thought of committing violence will begin to lead to this reaction, and the probability that the individual will behave violently will gradually decrease.

The activation of VIM also acts as a mediator in distinguishing between moral and conventional transgressions. The observation of moral transgressions – actions that consist in harming people – and the subsequent victims' distress cues will eventually lead to the development of the conditioned reflex that activates VIM. In turn, social transgressions that do not lead to harm but only consist in violating established social norms will not be associated with distress cues, which means that the corresponding experience will not lead to the development of the conditioned reflex. This is how the individual becomes capable of identifying moral transgressions in various actions. Of course, individuals without VIM can evaluate a moral transgression as a bad act if someone teaches them that it is bad. However, in their assessment, they will refer to the words of other people without experiencing an aversive reaction to causing harm.

To support the validity of his model, Blair cites the results of many studies. Children with a predisposition to psychopathy and adult psychopaths do show a poor ability to distinguish between moral and social transgressions. The same applies to children with conduct disorder. In addition, and in line with the VIM position, adult psychopaths show reduced comprehension of situations likely to induce guilt, although they show appropriate comprehension of happiness, sadness, and even complex social emotions such as embarrassment. Moreover, children and adults with psychopathy show pronounced impairment in processing sad and fearful facial and vocal expressions. What is important to note is that the ability to distinguish between moral and social transgressions is not associated with a bad upbringing and abuse in childhood[130][135][136][137][138][139][140][141][142][143][144][145][146][147].

Many other studies also support this model. For example, aggressive behavior from callous and unemotional traits (CU traits), the presence of which in children is a prerequisite for psychopathy in adulthood, is associated with a poor ability to recognize fearful facial expressions and fearful body postures[148]. Children with high scores of CU traits also experience problems in recognizing expressions of sadness, and children with high scores of conduct disorder – in recognizing expressions of fear[149]. Schizophrenics with a history of violent crime differ from non-violent schizophrenics in their lower ability to recognize facial expressions, especially expressions of fear[150]. People with high affective psychopathy scores were found to be less able to distinguish genuine distress cues from staged ones. At the same time, this effect did not extend to other emotions, such as happiness, anger, or disgust; it was specific to distress cues[151]. Even the most up-to-date research shows that difficulties in recognizing fear and sadness in childhood are associated with greater propensity for proactive aggression[152].

Finally, it is worth noting that psychopathy as a result of VIM dysfunction is a mental disorder by Wakefield's criteria: a condition is a disorder if it leads to harm to oneself or others and is associated with the failure of some internal mechanism to perform a function for which it was biologically designed (i.e., naturally selected)[153][154].

The VIM model does not provide a complete explanation of the nature of aggression regulation, so Blair later expanded it and developed the Integrated Emotion System (IES) model, which considers the neurophysiological aspects of this process[135]. However, it still confirms the presence of intraspecific aggression inhibitions in humans and gives a general idea of how they work[Author's note].

vim_en.png

▶ See also: The problem of indirect violence; The ability to experience empathy and take the perspective of other people in psychopathic individuals; Violence inhibitor dysfunction is a cause of serious problems in interpersonal relationships


IV. Neurophysiology and genetics of aggression regulation

Although the theory of the violence inhibition mechanism can explain a lot on its own, a more in-depth understanding of it requires an explanation of how this mechanism works from a neurophysiological and genetic perspective. Among other things, this is particularly important because it will allow us to identify a direction for the development of therapeutic approaches aimed at treating violence inhibitor dysfunction in individuals who suffer from it.


1. The role of serotonin in the inhibition of aggression and how this process can be activated

A study on moral judgments and behavior suggests that a mechanism similar to Blair's VIM operates for imagined harms. The neurotransmitter serotonin (5-HT) is responsible for the functioning of this mechanism, and the model developed in the study explains its parallel role in the inhibition of actual harm (in the case of aggression) and imagined harm (in the case of moral judgments)[155]. Many other studies also confirm the key role of serotonin in the modulation of aggression in animals and humans[5][156][157][158][159].

Various experiments conducted on mice and rats showed that some agonists of 5-HT1A and 5-HT1B receptors (these chemical compounds cause a biological response in receptors or, put simply, activate them) are able to suppress offensive aggression while not affecting defensive behavior or other forms of activity.

Drugs such as TFMPP and eltoprazine, when administered into the lateral ventricles of the brain, had a significant effect on reducing the aggressiveness of mice and rats in the resident-intruder paradigm while not affecting defensive behavior. This effect was associated with the activation of postsynaptic 5-HT1B receptors[160]. In limited human trials, eltoprazine resulted in some reduction of aggression in patients with dementia, psychotic disorders, and mental retardation, with minimal or no side effects[161]. Administration of a selective 5-HT1A agonist called F15599 into the ventro-orbital prefrontal cortex of male mice reduced the manifestation of intense elements of aggression, biting during attacks, as well as lateral threat postures (demonstrating aggressive intentions), without affecting non-intense manifestations of aggression and other forms of behavior[162].

Administration of the 5-HT1B agonist CP-94253 into the ventro-orbital prefrontal cortex of mice also reduced the frequency of attack bites and the manifestation of lateral threat postures[163]. The importance of 5-HT1B receptors in the inhibition of aggression was also demonstrated in an experiment where administration of the agonist anpirtoline reduced the manifestation of various forms of aggression in mice, including aggression from social interaction with an opponent and aggression from frustration[164]. A highly selective effect compared to other agonists was shown by the administration of the 5-HT1A agonist alnespirone to rats. Again, this did not affect the defensive behavior in the case when the individual encountered an aggressive conspecific, as well as other forms of behavior[165].

Importantly, experiments on the treatment of hostility and aggression in violent offenders with naratriptan, which is a full agonist of 5-HT1B/1D receptors and a partial agonist of 5-HT1A receptors, were once suggested[166]. And the similar drug zolmitriptan was successful in selectively reducing aggression in mice and attenuating alcohol-heightened aggression in humans[167][168]. It has also been suggested that vortioxetine, which is a full agonist of 5-HT1A receptors and a partial agonist of 5-HT1B receptors (like many antidepressants, it is also a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor), may be an effective anti-aggressive agent. This is supported, among other things, by preliminary results obtained on a small number of patients[159][169].

Administration of bioactive compounds of natural origin can lead to a decrease in aggressive behavior. For example, injection and oral administration of the herbal extract mixture Kamishoyosan reduce aggressiveness in mice. This effect is associated with the activation of 5-HT1A receptors and improvements in the regulation of the serotonergic system[170]. And oral administration of the Yokukansan mixture, also a 5-HT1A agonist, was tested in aggressive confrontations between mice and had a selective anti-aggressive effect on them. It was also found that the active ingredient in this mixture is geissoschizine methyl ether from the extract of Uncaria rhynchophylla[171]. Inhalation of linalool, a component of a large number of essential oils, which is a 5-HT1A agonist, also leads to a selective anti-aggressive effect in animals[172][173].

The serotonergic (5-HT) system, including 5-HT1A and 5-HT1B receptors, plays a key role in the modulation of aggression in various species, including humans. It should be noted that in the case of 5-HT1A receptors, the activation of postsynaptic receptors in the amygdala, frontal cortex, and hypothalamus leads to the inhibition of aggression, while the activation of presynaptic receptors inhibits the functioning of the 5-HT system itself, which, on the contrary, can lead to aggressive behavior[156].

▶ See also: The history of the development of anti-aggressive agents for clinical use


2. Association of impairments in some brain regions with a lack of aggression regulation

In the regulation of aggression, the main role is played by the amygdala, which is involved in the formation of emotions and conditioned reflex reactions, and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (or orbitofrontal cortex), which is involved in the decision-making process[135][157][158][174]. Together, they regulate the neural circuit that mediates reactive aggression (this circuit includes the medial hypothalamus and periaqueductal gray) and the subcortical systems that respond to threats (among them, the basal ganglia, including the striatum). Both impairments of the amygdala and the orbitofrontal cortex can lead to increased levels of reactive aggression. At the same time, the orbitofrontal cortex does not inhibit reactive aggression but only increases or decreases the chance of triggering this process, depending on the surrounding social signals. The neural circuit that mediates proactive aggression is regulated by the amygdala (it includes the temporal lobe, which processes information, as well as the striatum and premotor cortex, which are necessary for the implementation of actual behavior)[135][174].

Psychopaths are characterized by an increased level of proactive aggression. They also show impairments in empathic response and regulation of fear-related behavior, which are the result of amygdala dysfunction. But psychopaths are not like other patients with amygdala dysfunction. Other functions of the amygdala, such as the formation of stimulus-reward associations and certain aspects of social cognition, are only mildly or not impaired at all in individuals with psychopathy. The reason for this may be the presence of genetic anomalies, which, instead of leading to global disruption of the functioning of the amygdala, have a more selective effect, disrupting the function of specific neurotransmitters[135].

Based on the framework for understanding conduct disorder, we get the following: genetic factors lead to decreased amygdala responsiveness, which in turn reduces the empathic response, and this is the cause of aggression from CU traits, antisocial behavior, and instrumental aggression. Another cause of antisocial behavior and instrumental aggression, as well as under-regulated responses to social provocations and reactive aggression based on frustration, is an impairment in the ability to make decisions, which in turn comes from decreased responsiveness of the striatum and ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Genetic influence also plays a significant role here. In addition, the emergence of dysfunction in one of the brain regions is associated with dysfunction in others. Thus, with decreased responsiveness of the amygdala, one would expect decreased responsiveness of the striatum and ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Environmental factors such as trauma, exposure to violence, and neglect in upbringing only play a role in increased amygdala responsiveness, resulting in greater sensitivity to threats. However, even here, genetic influence is involved, as it is present in all the components considered by this framework[175].

From the neurophysiological evidence, it is also worth noting that measurement of the binding potential of 5-HT1B receptors using positron emission tomography demonstrated its lower levels in the anterior cingulate cortex, orbitofrontal cortex, and striatum in people with a high level of aggressive traits. In the case of the striatum, a connection with psychopathic traits was also found[176].

framework_eng.png


3. Genetics of aggression regulation

Various studies show dozens of genes that affect certain aspects of offensive aggression. However, it is evident that for its effective regulation, the number of genes and neurotransmitters involved in this process must be limited. And evidence ranging from evolutionary ancientry to pharmacological and clinical data points to the key role of the serotonergic (5-HT) system[156].

We will begin with the genes for 5-HT1A and 5-HT1B receptors (in humans, these are the HTR1A gene, localized on chromosome 5, and the HTR1B gene, localized on chromosome 6). Knockout of the 5-HT1B gene in mice resulted in increased aggressiveness in their behavior. Knockout of the 5-HT1A gene did not give such a result; however, this can be explained by the functional difference between the corresponding presynaptic and postsynaptic receptors, which we discussed earlier, as the gene knockout affects both types of receptors at once[156][177]. The fact that antisocial alcoholism (leading to impulsive and aggressive behavior) is associated with some alleles of the 5-HT1B gene is an example of clear evidence of the role of serotonin receptors in the modulation of aggression in humans. Compared with a control group of healthy individuals and non-aggressive alcoholics, antisocial alcoholism in Finns was significantly associated with the HTR1B H861C polymorphism and, to some extent, with the HTR1B D6S284 polymorphism. Both polymorphisms were also significantly associated with antisocial alcoholism in the studied American Indian tribe[178]. Also, the HTR1B rs11568817 genotypes were found to be different between groups of children with high and low scores of CU traits[179].

In addition to the 5-HT1A and 5-HT1B genes, it is also important to consider three more genes: TPH2, MAOA (also known as the “warrior gene”), and SLC6A4 (SERT). Their respective enzymes are involved in 5-HT synthesis in the brain. In the case of the TPH2 gene, the C1473G allele may play an important role. Mice of the 129×1/SvJ strain, homozygous for the 1473C (C/C) allele, were more aggressive than mice of the BALB/cJ strain, homozygous for the 1473G (G/G) allele. In the case of rats and silver foxes, in which low aggressiveness was achieved by selection, increased activity of the TPH enzyme as well as higher concentrations of serotonin and its metabolite 5-HIAA were observed[156][180][181]. It should also be noted that the knockout of the TPH2 gene in rats led to aggressive behavior from a decrease in the sensitivity of 5-HT1A receptors[182].

Mice with MAOA gene knockout showed increased aggressiveness and decreased 5-HIAA levels, which also indicated a decrease in the function of the 5-HT system. The same can be seen in humans as well. Males from one Dutch family with a point mutation in exon 8 of the MAOA gene showed an increased level of impulsive aggression. Moreover, polymorphisms in the promoter region of the MAOA gene were associated with antisocial alcoholism in German subjects. Many other studies also demonstrate that mutations in the MAOA gene are associated with abnormal male aggression[156][183][184][185][186][187][188].

The short variant (S) of the promoter region (5-HTTLPR) of the serotonin transporter gene SLC6A4 (SERT) leads to a decrease in the expression of the 5-HTT protein. For the homozygous allele (S/S), a significant association with aggressive behavior and a tendency for violence in humans has been shown in various samples: children, adults, adopted children, cocaine addicts, and patients with personality disorders[189][190][191][192][193][194].

5ht_eng.png


4. How genetics mediates environmental influences on aggressive behavior

One of the studies showed that the stability in reactive aggression from childhood to adolescence could be explained by genetics (48%) as well as shared (11%) and nonshared (41%) environmental influences, whereas the stability in proactive aggression was primarily genetically (85%) mediated[195]. According to a study of 7-year-old children, the presence of both сallous-unemotional traits and antisocial behavior has an extremely high level of heredity (81%). At the same time, if children have only antisocial behavior, a moderate level of heredity is observed (30%)[196].

The difference between these two groups of children lies in the fact that the members of the first group, who have сallous-unemotional traits, are capable of premeditated antisocial behavior and violence. They have a high risk of developing psychopathy and violent criminality in adulthood, and they are also difficult to educate. Their condition is more aggravated than the condition of children from the second group, whose behavior problems are not so serious and long-term. It is also worth mentioning that at older ages, a strong genetically mediated stability of сallous-unemotional traits is still preserved. Thus, between 17 and 24 years, it is 58%[197].

Interesting things were found in a study of the role of genotype in violence due to childhood abuse. Carriers of the high-activity allele of the MAOA gene did not become more violent than average. But carriers of the low-activity allele were 4 times more likely to commit rapes, robberies, and assaults in adulthood. Of course, by itself, the low-activity allele of this gene does not make a person more violent but creates such a risk depending on environmental influences[198][199].

An assessment of the psychopathy scores (using the PCL:YV and PCL-R tests) in male prisoners has shown that two to three times higher scores in adolescents and one and a half times higher scores in adults can be observed if they are carriers of the T/T genotype of the HTR1B-rs13212041 polymorphism, in comparison with carriers of the C/C and C/T genotypes. Also, childhood abuse additionally increases the risk of high levels of psychopathy in carriers of the T/T genotype by two to three times. As we can see, genetics in this case is a dynamic factor of development that mediates the influence of the environment[200].

A study of different variants of the serotonin transporter gene (SLC6A4, encoding the 5-HTT protein) in young people with different socioeconomic status (based on the income and professional status of their parents) showed that homozygosity for the long allele of this gene significantly increased the risk of narcissistic and callous-unemotional traits in the case of low status. At the same time, the short allele was associated with impulsive behavior (regardless of socioeconomic status). It seems that different alleles of this gene carry risks for different forms of aggression[201].


5. What approaches to the treatment of aggressive behavior are ineffective

Dopamine receptor antagonists (the drugs that block them) such as chlorpromazine and haloperidol are widely used in the treatment of aggressive patients, especially those with psychotic disorders. However, their effect is sedative, and in animals, they impair defensive behavior. This and other side effects limit their usefulness in the treatment of aggression. The use of barbiturates and benzodiazepines that affect GABA inhibitory neurotransmission faces the same problems. Beta-blockers such as propranolol and nadolol are effective in patients with organic brain syndromes and chronic psychosis, but they can also lead to side effects. Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) have shown efficacy in reducing aggressiveness in patients with borderline personality disorder, but they affect other behaviors and lead to unwanted side effects. Finally, agonists and antagonists of 5-HT2 receptors are also able to reduce aggressiveness; however, the former lead to side effects, while the influence of the latter is still poorly understood[202][203][204][205][206][207][208][209].

It should be noted that 5-HT2A agonists include many psychedelics known for their anti-aggressive and empathic effects. In one experiment, the administration of psilocybin even resulted in a sustained reduction in patients' predisposition to authoritarian political views. However, the administration of psychedelics leads to a number of side effects, and their empathic effect may be related to 5-HT1A receptors, for which they are often partial agonists[210][211][212][213].

Psychotherapeutic approaches have demonstrated the possibility of significantly reducing psychopathic predispositions and increasing affective (emotional) empathy. However, in some of their forms, particularly those aimed at restraining aggression by improving self-control, there is a risk that the problem will only worsen as the patient learns to commit acts of violence more effectively and thoughtfully. In addition, psychotherapy for such a severe disorder as psychopathy is too time-consuming; the number of therapeutic sessions required can reach dozens, and the entire process can last up to several years. It is worth highlighting the assumption that psychotherapy can be effective in dealing with violent youth and school bullies with high levels of psychopathy, but only at the most intensive level of intervention (at least 4 sessions per week for a year)[214][215][216][217]. Obviously, psychotherapy requires too much time and effort to rely solely on it[Author's note].

Based on this, 5-HT1A and 5-HT1B agonists seem to be the most promising drugs in the treatment of aggressive behavior since they have a highly selective effect without affecting defensive behavior or other forms of activity. This opinion is also shared by some researchers who support the resumption of research on the development of such anti-aggressive agents (or so-called “serenics”)[218]. They claim that “modern research suggests that aggressive behavior should be studied as a separate functional disorder” and “it is hoped that new insights into the neurobiology of aggression will reveal novel avenues for treatment of this destructive and costly behavior”[219][220]. There are also suggestions of therapy for aggressive and antisocial behavior through gene therapy, for example, targeting the MAOA gene[221].


6. The issue of parochial altruism: does oxytocin affect aggression

Oxytocin is a hormone that plays a crucial role in prosocial behaviors such as trust building, pair bonding, and mothering[222][223][224]. In intragroup relationships, oxytocin contributes to the establishment of altruism, uniting and coordinating the actions of individual members of the group. But as it is believed, in the case of intergroup interactions, this only increases aggressiveness since a cohesive group is ready to fight more fiercely with other, “alien” groups, to which members oxytocin does not stimulate altruism. The emergence of wars between different groups of people is often explained by this phenomenon called parochial altruism[124][125].

At first glance, this explanation does not fit well, if not completely contradicts the theory of the violence inhibition mechanism. But we can see that there is no contradiction at all if we look at how the two mechanisms interact at the neurophysiological level. The serotonergic system, including 5-HT1A and 5-HT1B receptors, is involved in the regulation of oxytocin secretion[225]. In one of the experiments, administration of the 5-HT1A agonist to mice not only had an anti-aggressive effect on them but also promoted prosocial behavior due to oxytocin secretion. Pretreatment with an oxytocin receptor antagonist (a drug that blocks it) suppressed prosocial behavior but did not reduce the anti-aggressive effect in any way[226].

It can be concluded that the regulation of aggression and stimulation of prosocial behavior, although significantly overlapping, are still different functions, explained by the work of different neurophysiological mechanisms. Therefore, there should not necessarily be a contradiction between the theory of the violence inhibition mechanism and the parochial altruism hypothesis[Author's note].


7. How the pro-aggressive effect of testosterone is being constrained

One study investigated the hypothesis that the pro-aggressive effect of the male sex hormone testosterone arises from the suppression of serotonergic system function and disproved it. Testosterone activation of aggression and serotonin inhibition of aggression work independently of each other. And the influence of serotonin on testosterone-induced aggression appears to be mediated by a parallel inhibitory pathway. It is assumed that this influence occurs in brain regions such as the medial amygdala, the hypothalamus, the prefrontal cortex, and the lateral septum, which are known to be involved in the regulation of aggression and where a high density of both sex steroid receptors and serotonergic nerve terminals is observed[227].

This influence from the serotonergic system is obviously necessary to restrain aggression so that it does not cease to be an adaptive and functional behavior. And since testosterone does not disrupt its function, it cannot be the cause of uninhibited aggression just by itself. Of course, it increases aggressiveness, but only within the natural inhibitory control[Author's note].


As studies show, the tendency to impulsive aggression can be explained by a dysfunctional interaction between the serotonergic and dopamine systems in the prefrontal cortex. At the same time, it is the lack of serotonin that predisposes a person to impulsive aggression, while dopamine hyperfunction aggravates this condition[228][229]. Serotonin is known to inhibit the production of dopamine, meaning low serotonin levels can lead to an overabundance of dopamine. Serotonin inhibits impulsive behavior, while dopamine promotes it by decreasing emotional regulation[230][231][232]. Increased dopamine activity due to serotonin dysfunction can also stimulate a person to search for new sensations, including by resorting to violent actions[233].

Also, one study directly links violent behavior in children and psychopathic predispositions in adults with altered dopamine system activity. And the cause of an excess of dopamine, which leads to increased aggressiveness, is a dysfunction of the serotonergic system, which should regulate the production of dopamine and inhibit aggressive impulses[234].

It is worth considering the case when a person has a low level of dopamine, for example, due to genetic predispositions. This should reduce the impulsiveness of behavior, and experiments on animals have demonstrated that blocking the production of dopamine decreases their aggressiveness upon contact with conspecifics[235]. But decreased dopamine level can lead a person to need more stimulation in order to experience the same level of pleasure. This encourages individuals to look for additional sources of pleasure, such as substance use, risky behavior, or even violence[236].

Research shows that impulse disorders such as pyromania and kleptomania are associated with the release of extra dopamine, and this can cause a person to become addicted to certain criminal activities over time. Similarly, this can work with serial killers, who seek the “ultimate thrill”[237][238]. It is also suggested that serial killers may need more stimulation in order to get the same pleasure that ordinary people or non-violent criminals experience[239][240].


V. The solution to the problem of violence and the social consequences of its eradication

With a direction for developing therapies aimed at correcting the dysfunction of the violence inhibition mechanism, we can make concrete proposals and consider how they can be implemented. It will also be important to look at what social changes the widespread practice of such therapies might lead to.

In order to prevent some misunderstandings, we should note that there is no question of changing the nature of a human being; here we consider only the question of therapy for a specific disorder and enhancement of neurophysiological functions already present in humans.

This Chapter largely demonstrates the author's ideas.


1. The solution to the problem of violence and potential areas of its application

We now know that many animals, and even humans, have an innate mechanism that can suppress offensive aggression without affecting defensive behavior or other forms of activity. Understanding the neurophysiological and genetic aspects of this mechanism can contribute to the development of several solutions aimed at both the temporary and permanent eradication of violence from the behavior of individuals by correcting and strengthening the function of the violence inhibition mechanism.

The most obvious solution is to create a pharmacological drug based on an agonist of 5-HT1A and/or 5-HT1B receptors that will have the most selective effect, activating the violence inhibitor without affecting other neurophysiological functions. This drug can be used in the treatment of patients suffering from increased aggressiveness. Research on 5-HT1A/1B agonists for this purpose has already been undertaken in the past, and some researchers are currently supporting its resumption. It can also be administered to violent criminals as an alternative to imprisonment or other forms of punishment and correction.

Perhaps a less selective but stronger and fast-acting version of such a drug can be used in some situations as a safe alternative to tranquilizers. It may be used when there is a need to quickly calm down an overly aggressive patient or remotely stop a person with obvious violent intentions using, for example, a dart gun or aerial spraying.

Another option is to create a gene therapy drug. This is a new approach to the treatment of congenital and genetically determined pathologies. The best-known example of gene therapy is the treatment of spinal muscular atrophy in children with the drug Zolgensma, which provides a new copy of the SMN1 gene[241]. There is also the suggestion of gene therapy for aggressive and antisocial behavior through targeting the MAOA gene[221].

To fix a dysfunctional violence inhibitor, we need to perform brain gene therapy, which is also a realistic task. For example, scientists have recently been able to develop gene therapy to treat a serious genetic disorder that leads to a lack of key neurotransmitters (dopamine and serotonin) and causes mental retardation, an inability to fully control body movements, decreased muscle tone, seizures, and other severe symptoms. During experimental treatment, seven patients aged four to nine years got rid of seizures, began to try to speak and smile, and two of them were even able to walk with assistance, which was previously considered fundamentally impossible for such a diagnosis[242]. Also, the possibility of gene therapy for neurological and neuropsychiatric disorders with a genetic origin is being actively studied[243].

At the moment, the most promising solution in brain gene therapy is the use of adeno-associated viral vectors as a deliverer of the correct gene variant to the necessary cells of the nervous system. One of the recent studies demonstrated the possibility of effective administration of such gene therapy to large mammals[244]. Another study showed how its use in the nucleus accumbens to restore the expression of the p11 protein gene that binds 5-HT1B and 5-HT4 receptors can help treat depression[245]. Also, in a mouse model of autism-like behavior, the administration of a vector with the 5-HT1A receptor gene into the hippocampus was tested to achieve its overexpression. Although this did not lead to an overall improvement in the condition, there was a significant reduction in anxiety in the tested individuals[246]. An alternative proposal could be to use a combination of the technologies CRISPR/Cas9, which allows the replacement of some DNA sequences with others, and iPSC, which consists in the creation and application of artificial stem cells, or any other similar combination[221].

We now need to consider methods for detecting dysfunction in the violence inhibition mechanism. An electrophysiological study of this mechanism in relation to aggressive traits showed their inverse association with the Stop-P300 amplitude responses to the observation of facial distress cues. And in relation to CU traits, it showed their inverse association with the N170 amplitude responses to the observation of all facial expressions. These amplitudes may provide useful electrophysiological markers for detecting impairments in the violence inhibitor function[15][247]. Through measuring the level of the serotonin metabolite 5-HIAA in the cerebrospinal fluid, it is possible to determine impairments in the serotonergic system leading to aggressive behavior[248]. Also, studies show the possibility of creating portable genetic tests, and low-cost portable genetic labs, such as Bento Lab, are already available for purchase[249][250]. Such tools can be very useful in diagnosing genetic predispositions to violence inhibitor dysfunction. And don't forget about questionnaires such as Robert Hare's “Psychopathy Checklist Revised” (PCL-R, screening version PCL:SV, and youth version PCL:YV), the Levenson Self-Report Psychopathy Scale, “The Inventory of Callous-Unemotional Traits,” and others, which also show good results in determining the presence of psychopathic traits.

In addition, some data allow us to create automated tools for tentative and preliminary detection of psychopathic predispositions. For example, when participating in an interview, individuals with them exhibit more stationary head positions, focused directly towards the camera or interviewer[251]. It may also be possible to create an AI solution that assesses an individual's potential psychopathy by a variety of physiological indicators that are associated with it, such as less tendency to “contagious” yawning, lower levels of respiratory sinus arrhythmia (heart rate changes in response to respiration), reduced heart rate in a calm state, and others[252][253][254].

Violence inhibitor function testing could be carried out even in children from a very early age. That small percentage of them with dysfunction after just one injection will undergo healthy socialization and be free of the risk of becoming violent individuals for life. The problem of violence will eventually be solved long before it occurs. Society will come closer to achieving free and non-violent order, where there is no place for either private manifestations of violence or violence as a method of governing society.

Of course, gene therapy technology is still at the beginning of development and too expensive, but in the future, it may become very affordable, as it was with many other technologies in the past. Now, the main part of the cost of any gene therapy drug is the cost of its development. However, the cost of creating each subsequent dose in mass production should decrease. It is unlikely that, in this case, the cost of the drug will be higher than the cost of modern vector vaccines.

Another potential solution concerns the defense sector. It is technically possible to create a drug based on a self-replicating viral vector that can be transmitted from individual to individual. At the moment, a similar concept is already being used in attempts to create so-called “contagious” vaccines. Once such vaccines were successfully applied to the rabbit population in the fight against two viral diseases[255][256].

Perhaps, based on the current knowledge of the violence inhibition mechanism, it is quite realistic to develop a biological solution that will be the most humane version of a strategic defensive weapon. It can be applied to a hostile army in the event of an attack to enhance the function of the violence inhibitor in its soldiers, resulting in a drastic reduction in its combat effectiveness. In view of the rapid decrease in the cost of biotechnologies, this solution may become available even to small countries that previously could not afford any serious weapons. For societies that have eradicated violence, it could become the primary weapon of deterrence against external threats.

We should understand that the actual application of such a biological solution is extremely risky and should be avoided, limited only to its use to deter potential aggressors from attacking. Though, in general, this is still a much more humane type of weapon than the already existing chemical, biological, or nuclear weapons.

gene.png

▶ See also: Proactive epigenesis: upbringing and education as a method of epigenetic fixation of non-violence; The influence of diet on the propensity for violence


2. Stages and social consequences of eradicating violence in society

The process of eradicating violence through therapeutic correction and strengthening the function of the violence inhibition mechanism in a minority of people with its deficiency will lead to a number of changes in society. It is very important to list these changes so that no one doubts the need to eradicate violence. It is also important to consider some of the contentious issues associated with this process so that this idea does not acquire any misconceptions.

Obviously, a positive and indisputable consequence of this is the solution of the problem of violent crime, which will radically reduce the level of stress in society and help to avoid human victims of violence, as well as the financial and material costs associated with it. This, together with the treatment of aggressive patients in medical institutions, are the very first areas in which this therapy should be applied. Even within the current social system, without the need to change it drastically, such an idea can be accepted as potentially the fastest, easiest, cheapest, and most effective way to solve relevant problems. This is the first stage that will demonstrate to the general public and popularize anti-violence therapy. After that, it will not be such a problem to move on to the second stage, which involves the widespread practice of testing the violence inhibitor in people.

A controversial point is the potential abuse of such therapy by governments. At first glance, by reducing the level of violence in society, they can selectively increase their violent potential by not applying such therapy to some of their agents. Some may even directly pursue the goal of suppressing the violence inhibitor in police officers and military personnel. However, in reality, the result will be the opposite. Governments recruit enforcers from society, and the lower the overall level of violence, the lower their ability to do this.

Ultimately, the following results can be expected:

– Governments will stop using violence to maintain social order and being “stationary bandits” that use their positions for their own benefit. They will have to replace violence with other methods, such as reputational and financial sanctions applied to citizens who violate social norms. Thus, a free non-violent society will be achieved, and the institution of statehood will either undergo radical changes, especially in terms of methods of conducting its activities, or be replaced by something more suitable to a free society;

– For the same reason, the unleashing of military conflicts will become simply impossible, a non-violent society will not tolerate this, and no one in it will be ready to participate in military attacks.

Another point of contention concerns the ability of non-violent individuals and societies to defend themselves against violent threats. But there is nothing to worry about:

– It must be remembered that defensive aggression or self-defense in the presence of an immediate threat to life is a natural form of behavior, the violence inhibition mechanism suppresses only offensive aggression and the desire to initiate harm to other people;

– The therapeutic eradication of violence will not be an instant process, creating a completely pacifist society surrounded by potential aggressors. It will take time, during which it can begin to spread around the world, leading to a gradual, multi-generational eradication of violence worldwide. International practice with the prospect of a global reduction in violence will be the third stage in the process of its eradication;

– A free non-violent society is able to protect itself from external threats with the help of modern weapons of deterrence, simply making itself an unprofitable victim. One of the proposed and available options for such a weapon could be a drug for enhancing the function of the violence inhibitor, working on the principle of a “contagious” vaccine. Of course, the actual application of such a biological solution to attacking armies is extremely risky and should be avoided by using it solely as a deterrent. However, this is still a much more humane type of weapon than the already existing chemical, biological, or nuclear weapons.

Finally, it is worth noting that in the modern high-tech world, there is a risk of using the achievements of scientific and technological progress for violent aspirations, including the use of weapons of mass destruction. An obvious example of this is nuclear weapons; however, the matter is not limited to them. The threat of bioterrorism using pathogens created in “basement labs” is already quite real, and it is not known what other threats await us in the future. The eradication of violence, in turn, will drastically reduce this risk. Perhaps this will even help to avoid the potential self-destruction of humanity. We should always remember what Konrad Lorenz pointed out: being the most armed species on the planet, we are also obliged to possess the strongest inhibitions of aggression.

pacifism.png

▶ See also: Moral bioenhancement: the only alternative to global totalitarianism and the destruction of humanity; The philosophy of biological voluntarism


References

Open the list of references


3), 3) Haller, J., & Kruk, M. R. (2006). Normal and abnormal aggression: human disorders and novel laboratory models. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 30(3), 292–303. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.01.005
4), 4), 4) Natarajan, D., de Vries, H., Saaltink, D.-J., de Boer, S. F., & Koolhaas, J. M. (2008). Delineation of Violence from Functional Aggression in Mice: An Ethological Approach. Behavior Genetics, 39(1), 73–90. doi:10.1007/s10519-008-9230-3
5), 5), 5) Natarajan, D. (2010). aSERTaining conflict in mice: https://pure.rug.nl/ws/portalfiles/portal/14508142/Deepa_FINAL.pdf
6) Miczek, K. A., Fish, E. W., DeBold, J. F., de Almeida, R. M. M. (2002). Social and neural determinants of aggressive behavior: pharmacotherapeutic targets at serotonin, dopamine and γ-aminobutyric acid systems. Psychopharmacology. 163:434–458. PubMed: 12373445
7) Miczek, K. A., Fish, E. W., De Bold, J. F. (2003). Neurosteroids, GABAA receptors, and escalated aggressive behavior. Horm Behav. 44:242–257. PubMed: 14609546
8) Natarajan. D., Caramaschi D. (2010). Animal violence demystified. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2010.00009
9) Takahashi, A., Quadros, I. M., de Almeida, R. M. M., & Miczek, K. A. (2011). Behavioral and Pharmacogenetics of Aggressive Behavior. Behavioral Neurogenetics, 73–138. doi:10.1007/7854_2011_191
10) [(Matthews, L. H. (1964). Overt fighting in mammals. In: Carthy, J. D., Ebling, F. J. (eds) The natural history of aggression. New York Academy Press, New York, pp 23–32
11), 11) Ohman, A. et al (2001). Emotion Drives Attention: Detecting the Snake in the Grass. Karolinska Institute
12), 12) Isaacs, S. (2016). How hardwired are we against threats?: An EDA study comparing modern and evolutionary fear-relevant stimuli. University of Skövde
13), 13) Subra, B. et al (2016). Of guns and snakes: testing a modern threat superiority effect. Cognition and Emotion, Volume 32, 81-91
14) Friedenberg, J. & Silverman, G. (2015). The Emotional Approach: Mind as Emotion. Cognitive Science, SAGE Publications, Inc
15), 15) Fido, D. (2015). Electrophysiological indices of the Violence Inhibition Mechanism and their associations with physical aggression, callous-unemotional traits, and dietary omega-3. Nottingham Trent University
16) Blanchard, R. J., Wall, P. M., Blanchard, D. C. (2003). Problems in the study of rodent aggression. Hormones and Behavior. 44(3):161–170
17) Koolhaas, J. M., Coppens, C. M., de Boer, S. F., Buwalda, B., Meerlo, P., & Timmermans, P. J. A. (2013). The Resident-intruder Paradigm: A Standardized Test for Aggression, Violence and Social Stress. Journal of Visualized Experiments, (77). doi:10.3791/4367
18) Dodge, K. A., Coie, J. D. (1987). Social-information-processing factors in reactive and proactive aggression in children’s peer groups. J Pers Soc Psychol. 53(6):1146–58
19) Euler, F., Steinlin, C., & Stadler, C. (2017). Distinct profiles of reactive and proactive aggression in adolescents: associations with cognitive and affective empathy. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health, 11(1). doi:10.1186/s13034-016-0141-4
20), 20) Gómez, J., Verdú, M., González-Megías, A. et al. (2016). The phylogenetic roots of human lethal violence. Nature 538, 233–237. doi:10.1038/nature19758
21), 21), 21), 21) Lorenz, K. (1949). Er redete mit dem Vieh, den Vögeln und den Fischen (King Solomon's Ring — 1961)
22), 22), 22), 22) Lorenz, K. (1963). Das sogenannte Böse. Zur Naturgeschichte der Aggression (On aggression — 2002)
23), 23), 23) Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1970). Ethology: The Biology of Behavior, pp. 314—325
24) Maynard, S. J., Price, G. R. (1973). “The logic of animal conflict.” Nature, 246:15-18
25) Hinde, R. A. (1974). Biological Bases of Human Social Behaviour. New York: McGraw-Hill
26) Blanchard, D. C., Blanchard, R. J. (1989). “Experimental animal models of aggression: What do they say about human behaviour?” in Archer, J.; Browne, K., Eds., Human Aggression: Naturalistic Approaches. London: Routledge, pp. 94-121.
27) Alcock, J. (2005). Animal Behavior: An Evolutionary Approach, eighth edition. Sunderland: Sinauer
28) Kokko, H. (2008). “Animal behavioral studies: Non-primates,” in Kurtz, L., Ed., Encyclopedia of Violence, Peace, and Conflict, Vol. 1, second edition. New York: Elsevier/Academic Press, pp. 47-56.
29), 29), 29) Fry, D. P., Schober, G., Björkqvist, K. (2010). Nonkilling As An Evolutionary Adaptation. In book: Nonkilling Societies (pp.101-128). Center for Global Nonkilling
30), 30), 30), 30), 30), 30) Miklikowska, M. & Fry, D. (2012). Natural Born Nonkillers A Critique of the Killers-Have-More-Kids Idea. In book: Nonkilling Psychology (pp.34-67)
31), 31) Hare, B. (2017). Survival of the Friendliest: Homo sapiens Evolved via Selection for Prosociality. Annual Review of Psychology, 68(1), 155–186. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-010416-044201
32) Pinker, S. (2011). The Better Angels of Our Nature: Why Violence Has Declined. ISBN: 978-0-670-02295-3
33) Ferguson, R. B. (2013). Pinker's list: Exaggerating prehistoric war mortality. In D. P. Fry (Ed.), War, peace, and human nature: The convergence of evolutionary and cultural views (pp. 112–131). Oxford University Press.
34), 34) Haas, J. & Piscitelli, M. (2013). The Prehistory of Warfare: Misled by Ethnography. Pp. 168-190 In: DP Fry ed. War peace and human nature: the convergence of evolutionary and cultural views. Oxford Univ. Press.
35) Ferguson, R. B. (1997). War before Civilization: The Myth of the Peaceful Savage. American Anthropologist, 99(2), 424-425
36), 36), 36) Ferguson, R. B. (2013). The prehistory of war and peace in Europe and the Near East. Pp. 191-240 In: DP Fry ed. War peace and human nature: the convergence of evolutionary and cultural views. Oxford Univ. Press.
37) Nakao, H., Tamura, K., Arimatsu, Y., Nakagawa, T., Matsumoto, N., & Matsugi, T. (2016). Violence in the prehistoric period of Japan: the spatio-temporal pattern of skeletal evidence for violence in the Jomon period. Biology Letters, 12(3), 20160028. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2016.0028
38), 38) Waller, L. (2023). Steven Pinker is WRONG About the Decline of Violence: https://www.thenandnow.co/2023/09/26/steven-pinker-is-wrong-about-the-decline-of-violence/
39) Fry, D. P. (2006). The human potential for peace. New York: Oxford University Press.
40) Fry, D. P. (2018). The Evolutionary Logic of Human Peaceful Behavior. Peace Ethology, 247–265. doi:10.1002/9781118922545.ch14
41) Moore, John H. (1990). “The reproductive success of Cheyenne war chiefs: A contrary case to Chagnon’s Yanomamö,” Current Anthropology, 31: 322-330
42) Beckerman, Stephen; Erickson, P.; Yost, J.; Regalado, J.; Jaramillo, L.; Sparks, C.; Iromenga, M. and Long, K. (2009). “Life histories, blood revenge, and reproductive success among the Waorani of Ecuador,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(20): 8134-8139
43) Knusel, C. J., & Outram, A. K. (2006). Fragmentation of the body: Comestibles, compost, or customary rite? In R. Gowland, & C. Knusel (Eds.), Social archaeology of funerary remains (pp. 253–276). Oxford: Oxbow Books
44), 44), 44), 44), 44), 44), 44), 44), 44), 44), 44) Grossman, D. (1995). On Killing: The Psychological Cost of Learning to Kill in War and Society. Boston: Little, Brown
45), 45), 45), 45) Van der Dennen, J. M. G. (2008). A vindication of Eibl-Eibesfeldt’s concept of Tötungshemmungen (conspecific killing inhibitions)? Human ethology, military psychology, and the neurosciences. University of Groningen, the Netherlands: https://web.archive.org/web/20240229203457/https://rint.rechten.rug.nl/rth/dennen/killinginhibitions.htm
46), 46), 46) Grossman, D., & Siddle, B. K. (2008). Psychological Effects of Combat*. Encyclopedia of Violence, Peace, & Conflict, 1796–1805. doi:10.1016/b978-012373985-8.00148-3
47) Swank, R. L. & Marchand, W. E. (1946). Combat neuroses: Development of combat exhaustion. Arch. Neurol. & Psychiat., 55, 3, pp. 236-247. doi:10.1001/archneurpsyc.1946.02300140067004
48) Marshall, S. L. A. (1947). Men Against Fire. New York: Morrow & Co. (Oklahoma Press edition, 2000. pp. 50-84)
49) Engen, R. (2008). Killing for Their Country: A New Look At 'Killology'. Vol. 9, No. 2. Canadian Military Journal: https://web.archive.org/web/20110721211742/http://www.journal.dnd.ca/vo9/no2/16-engen-eng.asp
50) Grossman, D. (2009). S.L.A. Marshall Revisited…? Vol. 9, No. 4. Canadian Military Journal: https://web.archive.org/web/20131105130149/http://www.journal.forces.gc.ca/vo9/18-grossman-eng.asp
51) Allard, S. (2022). On Grossman: How a Pseudoscientist Pushed Our Understanding of Killing Back 20 Years: https://web.archive.org/web/20231205024937/https://www.hosannafukuzawa.com/on-grossman-how-a-pseudoscientist-pushed-our-understanding-of-killing-back-20-years/
53) Lamb, R., Annetta, L., Hoston, D., Shapiro, M., Matthews, B. (2018). Examining human behavior in video games: The development of a computational model to measure aggression. Soc Neurosci. Jun;13(3):301-317. doi:10.1080/17470919.2017.1318777
54) Suziedelyte, A. (2021). Is it only a game? Video games and violence. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 188, pp. 105-125. doi: 10.1016/j.jebo.2021.05.014
55) Ghiglieri, M. P. (1999). The Dark Side of Man: Tracing the Origins of Male Violence. Reading, MA: Perseus Books
56) Ferguson, R. B. (2000). The Dark Side of Man: Tracing the Origins of Male Violence. American Anthropologist, 102(1), 172–173. doi:10.1525/aa.2000.102.1.172
57) Ardant du Picq, C. (1946). Battle studies. Harrisburg, Pa.: Telegraph Press
58) Keegan, J. & Holmes, R. (1985). Soldiers: A History of Men in Battle
59) Griffith, P. (1989). Battle tactics of the civil war. New Haven, Conn.: Yale University Press
60), 60), 60) Dyer, G. (1985). War. New York: Crown / War: The Lethal Custom. Carroll & Graf
61), 61) Gabriel, R. A. (1987). No more heroes: Madness and psychiatry in war. New York: Hill and Wang.
62) Suitt, T. H. (2021). High Suicide Rates among United States Service Members and Veterans of the Post-9/11 Wars: https://watson.brown.edu/costsofwar/files/cow/imce/papers/2021/Suitt_Suicides_Costs%20of%20War_June%2021%202021.pdf
63) Carney, T. (1989). The Unexpected Victory. In Karl D. Jackson, ed., Cambodia 1975–1978: Rendezvous With Death. Princeton University Press, pp. 13–35
64) The Crime of Cambodia: Shawcross on Kissinger's Memoirs New York Magazine, 5 November 1979
65) The Khmer Rouge National Army: Order of Battle, January 1976. New Haven: Yale University Cambodian Genocide Program, 1976
66) Locard, H. (2005). State Violence in Democratic Kampuchea (1975–1979) and Retribution (1979–2004). European Review of History: Revue Européenne D’histoire, 12(1), 121–143. doi:10.1080/13507480500047811
67) Population Division of the Department of Economic and Social Affairs of the United Nations Secretariat, World Population Prospects: The 2010 Revision
68) Kierman, B. (2023). The Demography of Genocide in Southeast Asia: The Death Tolls in Cambodia, 1975-79, and East Timor, 1975-80. Critical Asian Studies 35(4). doi:10.1080/1467271032000147041
69) Meierhenrich, J. (2020). “How Many Victims Were There in the Rwandan Genocide? A Statistical Debate”. Journal of Genocide Research. 22 (1): 72–82. doi:10.1080/14623528.2019.1709611
70) Guichaoua, A. (2020). “Counting the Rwandan Victims of War and Genocide: Concluding Reflections”. Journal of Genocide Research. 22 (1): 125–141. doi:10.1080/14623528.2019.1703329
71), 71) Jones, B. D. (2001). Peacemaking in Rwanda: the dynamics of failure
72), 72) Straus, S. (2004). How many perpetrators were there in the Rwandan genocide? An estimate. Journal of Genocide Research (2004), 6(1), March, 85–98
73) Nyseth Nzitatira, H., Edgerton, J. F., & Frizzell, L. C. (2022). Analyzing Participation in the 1994 Genocide in Rwanda. Journal of Peace Research. doi:10.1177/00223433221075211
74) Klemensits, P., Czirjak, R. (2016). Child Soldiers in Genocidal Regimes: The Cases of the Khmer Rouge and the Hutu Power. AARMS Vol. 15, No. 3 (2016) 215–222.
75) Cros, M-F. (1994). Jean Birara: ‘The Belgians and French Could Have Stopped the Killing’. Rosslyn: Foreign Broadcast Information Service (FBIS)
78) Falk, Ö., Wallinius, M., Lundström, S., Frisell, T., Anckarsäter, H., & Kerekes, N. (2013). The 1% of the population accountable for 63% of all violent crime convictions. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 49(4), 559–571. doi:10.1007/s00127-013-0783-y
79) Ulmer, J., & Steffensmeier, D. (2014). The age and crime relationship: Social variation, social explanations. SAGE Publications Ltd. doi:10.4135/9781483349114
80) Hildebrand, M., & de Ruiter, C. (2004). PCL-R psychopathy and its relation to DSM-IV Axis I and II disorders in a sample of male forensic psychiatric patients in the Netherlands. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 27(3), 233–248. doi:10.1016/j.ijlp.2004.03.005
81) Dåderman, A. M., & Hellström, Å. (2018). Interrater Reliability of Psychopathy Checklist–Revised: Results on Multiple Analysis Levels for a Sample of Patients Undergoing Forensic Psychiatric Evaluation. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 45(2), 234–263. doi:10.1177/0093854817747647
82) DeLisi, M., Peters, D. J., Hochstetler, A., Butler, H. D., Vaughn, M. G. (2023). Psychopathy among condemned capital murderers. J Forensic Sci. 68: 558– 567. doi:10.1111/1556-4029.15188
83) Fox, B., & DeLisi, M. (2018). Psychopathic killers: A meta-analytic review of the psychopathy-homicide nexus. Aggression and Violent Behavior. doi:10.1016/j.avb.2018.11.005
84) Coid, J., Yang, M., Ullrich, S., Roberts, A., & Hare, R. D. (2009). Prevalence and correlates of psychopathic traits in the household population of Great Britain. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 32(2), 65–73. doi:10.1016/j.ijlp.2009.01.002
85) Neumann, C. S., & Hare, R. D. (2008). Psychopathic traits in a large community sample: Links to violence, alcohol use, and intelligence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76(5), 893–899. doi:10.1037/0022-006x.76.5.893
86) Hare, R. D. (1999). Psychopathy as a risk factor for violence. Psychiatric Quarterly, 70(3), 181–197. doi:10.1023/a:1022094925150
87) Gatner, D. T., Douglas, K. S., Almond, M. F. E., Hart, S. D., & Kropp, P. R. (2023). How much does that cost? Examining the economic costs of crime in North America attributable to people with psychopathic personality disorder. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 14(4), 391–400. doi:10.1037/per0000575
88) Babiak, P., Neumann, C. S., & Hare, R. D. (2010). Corporate psychopathy: Talking the walk. Behavioral Sciences & the Law. doi:10.1002/bsl.925
89) Corporate psychopaths common and can wreak havoc in business, researcher says. (2016): https://web.archive.org/web/20231026114033/https://psychology.org.au/news/media_releases/13september2016/brooks
90) Boddy, C. R., Ladyshewsky, R. K., & Galvin, P. (2010). The Influence of Corporate Psychopaths on Corporate Social Responsibility and Organizational Commitment to Employees. Journal of Business Ethics, 97(1), 1–19. doi:10.1007/s10551-010-0492-3
91) Lesha, J., & Lesha, D. (2012). Psychopathy and white collar crime: A riview of literature. South East European University Review, 8(2). doi:10.2478/v10306-012-0017-2
93) Hare, R. D., León-Mayer, E., Salinas, J. R., Folino, J., Neumann, C. S. (2022). Psychopathy and crimes against humanity: A conceptual and empirical examination of human rights violators, Journal of Criminal Justice, Volume 81. doi:10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2022.101901
94) Cornell, D. G., Warren, J., Hawk, G., Stafford, E., Oram, G., & Pine, D. (1996). Psychopathy in instrumental and reactive violent offenders. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 783–790. doi:10.1037/0022-006x.64.4.783
95) Glenn, A. L., & Raine, A. (2009). Psychopathy and instrumental aggression: Evolutionary, neurobiological, and legal perspectives. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 32(4), 253–258. doi:10.1016/j.ijlp.2009.04.002
96) Porter, S., & Woodworth, M. (2007). “I’m sorry I did it…but he started it”: A comparison of the official and self-reported homicide descriptions of psychopaths and non-psychopaths. Law and Human Behavior, 31(1), 91–107. doi:10.1007/s10979-006-9033-0
97) Calhoun, J. (1973). Death Squared: The Explosive Growth and Demise of a Mouse Population. Proc. roy. Soc. Med. Vol. 66, no. 2. P. 80—88.
98) Хохловский, П. (2020). «Вселенная 25: разгромная критика мифов и новые выводы»: https://web.archive.org/web/20220909184613/https://tjournal.ru/analysis/212316-vselennaya-25-razgromnaya-kritika-mifov-i-novye-vyvody
99) Milgram, S. (1963). Behavioral Study of Obedience. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, № 67., p. 376
100) Thomas, B. (2004). The Man Who Shocked the World: The Life and Legacy of Stanley Milgram. Basic Books. ISBN 0-7382-0399-8
101) Perry, G. (2012). Behind the Shock Machine: the untold story of the notorious Milgram psychology experiments
102) Hollander, M. M., & Turowetz, J. (2017). Normalizing trust: Participants’ immediately post-hoc explanations of behaviour in Milgram’s ’obedience’ experiments. British Journal of Social Psychology, 56(4), 655–674. doi:10.1111/bjso.12206
103) Dolan, E. W. (2019). Unpublished data from Stanley Milgram’s experiments cast doubt on his claims about obedience. Social Psychology
104), 104) Эффект Люцифера. Ложь психологических экспериментов о насилии (2023): https://web.archive.org/web/20230602195506/https://telegra.ph/EHffekt-Lyucifera-Lozh-psihologicheskih-ehksperimentov-o-nasilii-01-01
105) Le Texier, T. (2019). Debunking the Stanford Prison Experiment. American Psychologist, 74(7), 823–839. doi:10.1037/amp0000401
108) Bianco, M. (2014). One Group Has a Higher Domestic Violence Rate Than Everyone Else — And It's Not the NFL: https://www.mic.com/articles/106886/one-group-has-a-higher-domestic-violence-rate-than-everyone-else-and-it-s-not-the-nfl
109) Morris, B. (2014). The Rate of Domestic Violence Arrests Among NFL Players: https://fivethirtyeight.com/features/the-rate-of-domestic-violence-arrests-among-nfl-players/
110) King, S. (2015). Police officers three times more likely to commit domestic violence than active NFL players: https://www.dailykos.com/stories/2015/9/10/1420117/-Police-officers-have-a-rate-of-domestic-violence-at-least-300-higher-than-active-players-in-the-NFL
111) Johnson, L. B. (1991). On the front lines: Police stress and family well-being. Hearing before the Select Committee on Children, Youth, and Families House of Representatives: 102 Congress First Session May 20 (p. 32-48). Washington DC: US Government Printing Office.
112) Neidig, P. H., Russell, H. E. & Seng, A. F. (1992). Interspousal aggression in law enforcement families: A preliminary investigation. Police Studies, Vol. 15 (1), p. 30-38.
113) Altevogt, R. (1957) Untersuchungen zur Biologie, Ökologie und Physiologie Indischer Winkerkrabben. Zeitschrift für Morphologie und Ökologie der Tiere 46(1):1–110
114) Crane, J. (1966). Combat, Display and Ritualization in Fiddler Crabs (Ocypodidae, Genus Uca). Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 251(772), 459–472. doi:10.1098/rstb.1966.0035
115) Walther, F. R. (1958). Zum Kampf- und Paarungsverhalten einiger Antilopen doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.1958.tb00570.x
116) Дольник, В. Р. (1993). Этологические экскурсии по запретным садам гуманитариев: https://web.archive.org/web/20210515080443/http://vivovoco.astronet.ru/VV/PAPERS/ECCE/VV_EH4_W.HTM
117) Langley, L. (2018). How Jellyfish Rule the Seas Without a Brain: https://www.nationalgeographic.co.uk/animals/2018/08/how-jellyfish-rule-the-seas-without-a-brain
118) Emergence of a Peaceful Culture in Wild Baboons. (2004). PLoS Biology, 2(4), e124. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0020124
119) Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behaviour. I. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7(1), 1–16. doi:10.1016/0022-5193(64)90038-4
122) Kim, S. S. (1976). The Lorenzian Theory of Aggression and Peace Research: A Critique. Journal of Peace Research, Vol. 13, No. 4 (1976), pp. 253-276.
123) Kudryavtseva, N. N. (2004). Lorenz Was Right! Or Does Aggressive Energy Accumulate? Russian Journal of Genetics, 40(6), 656–662. doi:10.1023/b:ruge.0000033313.41606.23
124), 124) Марков, А. (2010). Окситоцин усиливает любовь к «своим», но не улучшает отношения к чужакам. Элементы большой науки: https://web.archive.org/web/20221127134212/https://elementy.ru/novosti_nauki/431346/Oksitotsin_usilivaet_lyubov_k_svoim_no_ne_uluchshaet_otnosheniya_k_chuzhakam
125), 125) De Dreu, C. K. W. et al. (2010). The Neuropeptide Oxytocin Regulates Parochial Altruism in Intergroup Conflict Among Humans. Science. V. 328. P. 1408–1411. doi:10.1126/science.1189047
126) Archer, J. (1988). The Behavioural Biology of Aggression. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
127) Archer, J. & Huntingford, F. (1994). “Game theory models and escalation of animal fighting,” in Potegal, M., and Knutson, J. F., Eds., The Dynamics of Aggression: Biological and Social Processes in Dyads and Groups. Hillsdale: Erlbaum, pp. 3-31
128) Riechert, S. E. (1998). “Game theory and animal contests,” in Dugatkin, L. A. and Reeve, H. K., Eds., Game Theory and Animal Behavior. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 64-93
129) Blair, R. J. R. (1992/1993). The Development of Morality. Department of Psychology, University College, London
130), 130), 130) Blair, R. J. R. (1995). A cognitive developmental approach to morality: investigating the psychopath. Cognition 57, 1-29. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(95)00676-p
131) Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R.A., Bustamante, D., Mathy, R.M., Miller, P.A., & Lindholm, E. (1988). Differentiation of vicariously induced emotional reactions in children. Developmental Psychology, 24, 237-246
132) Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R.A., Carlo, G., Troyer, D., Speer, A.L., Karbon, M., & Switzer, G. (1992). The relations of maternal practices and characteristics to children's vicarious emotional responsiveness. Child Development, 63, 583-602.
133) Fabes, R.A., Eisenberg, N., & Eisenbud, L. (1993). Behavioral and physiological correlates of children's reactions to others in distress. Developmental Psychology, 29, 655-663.
134) Camras, L.A. (1977). Facial expressions used by children in a conflict situation. Child Development, 48, 1431-1435
135), 135), 135), 135), 135) Blair, R. J. R. (2006). The emergence of psychopathy: Implications for the neuropsychological approach to developmental disorders. Cognition 101, 414—442. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2006.04.005
136) Blair, R. J. R. (1997) Moral reasoning in the child with psychopathic tendencies. Personality and Individual Differences, 22, 731—739.
137) Blair, R. J. R., Jones, L., Clark, F., & Smith, M. (1995). Is the psychopath ‘‘morally insane’’?. Personality and Individual Differences 19, 741—752
138) Blair, R. J. R., Sellars, C., Strickland, I., Clark, F., Williams, A. O., Smith, M., et al. (1995). Emotion attributions in the psychopath. Personality and Individual Differences, 19, 431—437
139) Blair, R. J. R., Colledge, E., & Mitchell, D. G. (2001). Somatic markers and response reversal: is there orbitofrontal cortex dysfunction in boys with psychopathic tendencies?. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 29, 499—511. doi:10.1023/a:1012277125119
140) Blair, R. J. R., Colledge, E., Murray, L., & Mitchell, D. G. (2001). A selective impairment in the processing of sad and fearful expressions in children with psychopathic tendencies. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 29, 491—498. doi:10.1023/a:1012225108281
141) Blair, R. J. R., Monson, J., & Frederickson, N. (2001). Moral reasoning and conduct problems in children with emotional and behavioural difficulties. Personality and Individual Differences, 31, 799—811. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00181-1
142) Arsenio, W. F., & Fleiss, K. (1996). Typical and behaviourally disruptive children’s understanding of the emotion consequences of socio—moral events. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 14, 173—186.
143) Nucci, L. P., & Herman, S. (1982). Behavioral disordered children’s conceptions of moral, conventional, and personal issues. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 10, 411—425. doi:10.1007/BF00912330
144) Aniskiewicz, A. S. (1979). Autonomic components of vicarious conditioning and psychopathy. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 35, 60—67. doi:10.1002/1097-4679(197901)35:1<60::aid-jclp2270350106>3.0.co;2-r
145) Blair, R. J. R., Mitchell, D. G., Richell, R. A., Kelly, S., Leonard, A., Newman, C., et al. (2002). Turning a deaf ear to fear: impaired recognition of vocal affect in psychopathic individuals. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 682—686. doi:10.1037/0021-843x.111.4.682
146) Blair, R. J. R. (1999). Responsiveness to distress cues in the child with psychopathic tendencies. Personality and Individual Differences, 27, 135—145
147) House, T. H., & Milligan, W. L. (1976). Autonomic responses to modeled distress in prison psychopaths. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 556—560. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.34.4.556
148) Muñoz, L. C. (2009). Callous-Unemotional Traits Are Related to Combined Deficits in Recognizing Afraid Faces and Body Poses. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 48(5), 554–562
149) Woodworth, M., & Waschbusch, D. (2008). Emotional processing in children with conduct problems and callous/unemotional traits. Child: Care, Health and Development, 34(2), 234–244
150) Frommann, N., Stroth, S., Brinkmeyer, J., Wölwer, W., & Luckhaus, C. (2013). Facial Affect Recognition Performance and Event-Related Potentials in Violent and Non-Violent Schizophrenia Patients. Neuropsychobiology, 68(3), 139–145
151) Dawel, A., Wright, L., Dumbleton, R., & McKone, E. (2019). All tears are crocodile tears: Impaired perception of emotion authenticity in psychopathic traits. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 10(2), 185-197
152) Acland, E. L., Peplak, J., Suri, A., Malti, T. (2023). Emotion recognition links to reactive and proactive aggression across childhood: A multi-study design. Development and Psychopathology. doi:10.1017/S0954579423000342
153) Wakefield, J. C. (2007). The concept of mental disorder: diagnostic implications of the harmful dysfunction analysis. World Psychiatry. Oct;6(3):149-56. PMID: 18188432; PMCID: PMC2174594
154) Faucher, L. (2012). Evolutionary Psychiatry and Nosology: Prospects and Limitations. Baltic International Yearbook of Cognition, Logic and Communication. 7. doi:10.4148/biyclc.v7i0.1776
155) Siegel, J. Z., Crockett, M. J. (2013). How serotonin shapes moral judgment and behavior. Ann N Y Acad Sci. Sep;1299(1):42-51. doi:10.1111/nyas.12229
156), 156), 156), 156), 156), 156) Popova, N. K. (2006). From genes to aggressive behavior: the role of serotonergic system. BioEssays, 28(5), 495–503. doi:10.1002/bies.20412
157), 157) Blair, R. J. R., & Charney, D. S. (2003). Emotion Regulation. In: Mattson, M.P. (eds) Neurobiology of Aggression. Contemporary Neuroscience. Humana Press, Totowa, NJ. doi:10.1007/978-1-59259-382-8_2
158), 158) Rosell, D., & Siever, L. (2015). The neurobiology of aggression and violence. CNS Spectrums, 20(3), 254-279. doi:10.1017/S109285291500019X
159), 159) Popova, N. K., Tsybko, A. S., Naumenko, V. S. (2022). The Implication of 5-HT Receptor Family Members in Aggression, Depression and Suicide: Similarity and Difference. Int J Mol Sci., 23(15):8814. doi:10.3390/ijms23158814
160) Olivier, B. (2006). Serotonin and Aggression. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1036(1), 382–392. doi:10.1196/annals.1330.022 
161) Olivier, B., Mos, J., Raghoebar, M., de Koning, P., Mak, M. (1994). Serenics. Prog Drug Res. 42:167-308. doi:10.1007/978-3-0348-7153-2_6
162) Stein, D. J., Miczek, K. A., Lucion, A. B., & de Almeida, R. M. M. (2013). Aggression-reducing effects of F15599, a novel selective 5-HT1A receptor agonist, after microinjection into the ventral orbital prefrontal cortex, but not in infralimbic cortex in male mice
163) De Almeida, R. M. M., Rosa, M. M., Santos, D. M., Saft, D. M., Benini, Q., & Miczek, K. A. (2006). 5-HT1B receptors, ventral orbitofrontal cortex, and aggressive behavior in mice. Psychopharmacology, 185(4), 441–450. doi:10.1007/s00213-006-0333-3
164) De Almeida, R. (2002). Aggression Escalated by Social Instigation or by Discontinuation of Reinforcement (“Frustration”) in Mice Inhibition by Anpirtoline: A 5-HT1B Receptor Agonist. Neuropsychopharmacology, 27(2), 171–181. doi:10.1016/s0893-133x(02)00291-9
165) Boer, Sietse & Lesourd, M & Mocaer, E & Koolhaas, J. (1999). Selective Antiaggressive Effects of Alnespirone in Resident-Intruder Test Are Mediated via 5-Hydroxytryptamine1A Receptors: A Comparative Pharmacological Study with 8-Hydroxy-2-Dipropylaminotetralin, Ipsapirone, Buspirone, Eltoprazine, and WAY-100635
166) Van der Loo, A., Van Ojen, R., Koerselman, F., Nijman, H., Olivier, B. (2006). Efficacy of a Triptan in the Treatment of Hostility and Aggression Among Convicts With a Psychiatric Treatment Order: https://ctv.veeva.com/study/efficacy-of-a-triptan-in-the-treatment-of-hostility-and-aggression-among-convicts-with-a-psychiatric
167) De Almeida, R., Nikulina, E., Faccidomo, S., Fish, E., & Miczek, K. (2001). Zolmitriptan - a 5-HT 1B/D agonist, alcohol, and aggression in mice. Psychopharmacology, 157(2), 131–141. doi:10.1007/s002130100778
168) Gowin, J. L., Swann, A. C., Moeller, F. G., & Lane, S. D. (2010). Zolmitriptan and human aggression: interaction with alcohol. Psychopharmacology, 210(4), 521–531. doi:10.1007/s00213-010-1851-6
169) Marchiafava, M., Piccirilli, M., Bedetti, C., Baglioni, A., Menna, M., Elisei, S. (2018). Effectiveness of serotonergic drugs in the management of problem behaviors in patients with neurodevelopmental disorders. Psychiatr. Danub. 30, 644–647.
170) Igarashi, K., Kuchiiwa, T., Kuchiiwa, S., Iwai, H., Tomita, K., & Sato, T. (2021). Kamishoyosan (a Japanese traditional herbal formula), which effectively reduces the aggressive biting behavior of male and female mice, and potential regulation through increase of Tph1, Tph2, and Esr2 mRNA levels. Brain Research, 1768, 147580. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2021.147580
171) Nishi, A., Yamaguchi, T., Sekiguchi, K., Imamura, S., Tabuchi, M., Kanno, H., … Kase, Y. (2012). Geissoschizine methyl ether, an alkaloid in Uncaria hook, is a potent serotonin1A receptor agonist and candidate for amelioration of aggressiveness and sociality by yokukansan. Neuroscience, 207, 124–136. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.01.037
172) Linck, V. M., da Silva, A. L., Figueiró, M., Caramão, E. B., Moreno, P. R. H., & Elisabetsky, E. (2010). Effects of inhaled Linalool in anxiety, social interaction and aggressive behavior in mice. Phytomedicine, 17(8-9), 679–683. doi:10.1016/j.phymed.2009.10.002
173) Guzmán-Gutiérrez, S. L., Bonilla-Jaime, H., Gómez-Cansino, R., & Reyes-Chilpa, R. (2015). Linalool and β-pinene exert their antidepressant-like activity through the monoaminergic pathway. Life Sciences, 128, 24–29. doi:10.1016/j.lfs.2015.02.021
174), 174) Blair, R. J. R. (2004). The roles of orbital frontal cortex in the modulation of antisocial behavior. Brain and Cognition, 55(1), 198–208. doi:10.1016/s0278-2626(03)00276-8
175) Blair, R. J. R. (2013). The neurobiology of psychopathic traits in youths. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14(11), 786–799
176) Da Cunha-Bang, S., Hjordt, L. V., Perfalk, E., Beliveau, V., Bock, C., Lehel, S., … Knudsen, G. M. (2017). Serotonin 1B Receptor Binding Is Associated With Trait Anger and Level of Psychopathy in Violent Offenders. Biological Psychiatry, 82(4), 267–274. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.02.03
177) Saudou, F., Amara, D., Dierich, A., LeMeur, M., Ramboz, S., Segu, L., … Hen, R. (1994). Enhanced aggressive behavior in mice lacking 5-HT1B receptor. Science, 265(5180), 1875–1878. doi:10.1126/science.8091214
178) Lappalainen, J., Long, J. C., Eggert, M., Ozaki, N., Robin, R. W., Brown, G. L., … Goldman, D. (1998). Linkage of Antisocial Alcoholism to the Serotonin 5-HT1B Receptor Gene in 2 Populations. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55(11), 989. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.55.11.989
179) Moul, C., Dobson-Stone, C., Brennan, J., Hawes, D., & Dadds, M. (2013). An Exploration of the Serotonin System in Antisocial Boys with High Levels of Callous-Unemotional Traits. PLoS ONE, 8(2), e56619. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0056619
180) Popova, N. K., Voitenko, N. N., Kulikov, A. V., Avgustinovich, D. F. (1991). Evidence for the involvement of central serotonin in mechanism of domestication of silver foxes. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 40:751–756.
181) Zhang, X., Beaulieu, J. M., Sotnikova, T. D., Gainetdinov, R. R., Caron, M. G. (2004). TPH-2 controls brain serotonin synthesis. Science 305:217.
182) Peeters, Deborah & Terneusen, Anneke & Boer, Sietse & Newman-Tancredi, Adrian & Varney, Mark & Verkes, Robbert-Jan & Homberg, Judith. (2019). Enhanced aggressive phenotype of Tph2 knockout rats is associated with diminished 5-HT1A receptor sensitivity. Neuropharmacology. 153. doi:10.1016/j.neuropharm.2019.05.004
183) Sohrabi, S. (2015). The criminal gene: the link between MAOA and aggression (REVIEW). BMC Proceedings, 9(Suppl 1), A49. doi:10.1186/1753-6561-9-s1-a49
184) Cases, O., Seif, I., Grimsby, J., Gaspar, P., Chen, K., et al. (1995). Aggressive behavior and altered amounts of brain serotonin and norepinephrine in mice lacking MAOA. Science 268:1763–1766.
185) Popova, N. K., Gilinsky, M. A., Amstislavskaya, T. G., Morosova, E. A., Seif, I., et al. (2001). Regional 5-HT metabolism in the brain of transgenic mice lackingmonoamine oxidase A. J Neurosci Res 66:423–427.
186) Brunner, H. G., Nelen, M., Breakefield, X. O., Ropers, H. H., Van Oost, B. A. (1993). Abnormal behavior associated with a point mutation in the structural gene for monoamine oxidase A. Science 262:578–580.
187) Brunner, H. G. (1995). Monoamine oxidase and behavior. Ann Med 27:431–432.
188) Samochowiec, J., Lesch, K. P., Rottmann, M., Smolka, M., et al. (1999). Association of a regulatory polymorphism in the prompter region of the monoamine oxidase A gene with antisocial alcoholism. Psychiatry Res 86:67–72.
189) Ткаченко О. Н. (2016). Генетические корреляты агрессивности у человека: обзор литературы. Соц.-экол. технологии. 3:68-86.
190) Davidge, K.M., Atkinson, L., Douglas, L. (2004). Association of the serotonin transporter and 5HT1Dbeta receptor genes with extreme, persistent and pervasive aggressive behaviour in children. Psychiatr. Genet. 2004. V. 14. № 3. Pp. 143–146.
191) Sysoeva, O. V., Maluchenko, N. V., Timofeeva M. A. (2009). Aggression and 5HTT polymorphism in females: study of synchronized swimming and control groups. Int. J. Psychophysiol. V. 72. № 2. Pp. 173–178.
192) Cadoret, R. J., Langbehn, D., Caspers, K. (2003). Associations of the serotonin transporter promoter polymorphism with aggressivity, attention deficit, and conduct disorder in an adoptee population. Compr. Psychiatry. 2003. V. 44. № 2. Pp. 88–101.
193) Patkar, A. A, Berrettini, W. H., Hoehe, M. (2002). Serotonin transporter polymorphisms and measures of impulsivity, aggression, and sensation seeking among African-American cocaine-dependent individuals. Psychiatry Res. 2002. V. 110. № 2. Pp. 103–115.
194) Retz, W., Retz-Junginger, P., Supprian, T. (2004). Association of serotonin transporter promoter gene polymorphism with violence: Relation with personality disorders, impulsivity, and childhood ADHD psychopathology. Behav. Sci. Law. 2004. V. 22. № 3. Pp. 415–425.
195) Tuvblad, C., Raine, A., Zheng, M., & Baker, L. A. (2009). Genetic and environmental stability differs in reactive and proactive aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 35(6), 437–452. doi:10.1002/ab.20319
196) Viding, E., Blair, R. J. R., Moffitt, T. E., & Plomin, R. (2005). Evidence for substantial genetic risk for psychopathy in 7-year-olds. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(6), 592–597. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00393.x
197) Viding, E., Mccrory, E. (2013). Genetic Biomarker Research of Callous-Unemotional Traits in Children: Implications for the Law and Policymaking. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199844180.003.0009
198) Caspi, A. (2002). Role of Genotype in the Cycle of Violence in Maltreated Children. Science, 297(5582), 851–854. doi:10.1126/science.1072290
199) Nordio, E. (2011). Could an antisocial behaviour be influenced by our genes: http://www.antoniocasella.eu/nume/Nordio_2011.pdf
200) Palumbo, S., Mariotti, V., Vellucci, S., Antonelli, K., Anderson, N., Harenski, C., Pietrini, P., Kiehl, K. A., Pellegrini, S. (2022). HTR1B genotype and psychopathy: Main effect and interaction with paternal maltreatment. Psychoneuroendocrinology. Volume 144. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2022.105861
201) Sadeh, N., Javdani, S., Jackson, J. J., Reynolds, E. K., Potenza, M. N., Gelernter, J., … Verona, E. (2010). Serotonin transporter gene associations with psychopathic traits in youth vary as a function of socioeconomic resources. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119(3), 604–609. doi:10.1037/a0019709
202) Olivier, B., Mos, J., Hartog, J., & Rasmussen, D. L. (1990). Serenics: A new class of drugs for putative selective treatment of pathological destructive behaviour. Drug News & Perspectives, 3, 261–271.
203) Haller, J., Makara, G. B., & Kruk, M. R. (1998). Catecholaminergic involvement in the control of aggression: Hormones, the peripheral sympathetic and central noradrenergic systems. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 22(1), 85–97
204) Nelson, R. J., & Trainor, B. C. (2007). Neural mechanisms of aggression. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 536–546.
205) Fuller, R. W. (1996). The influence of fluoxetine on aggressive behavior. Neuropsychopharmacology, 14, 77–81.
206) Coccaro, E. F., & Kavoussi, R. J. (1997). Fluoxetine and impulsive aggressive behavior in personality disordered subjects. Archives of General Psychiatry, 54, 1081–1088.
207) Coccaro, E. F., Kavoussi, R. J., & Hauger, R. L. (1997). Serotonin function and antiaggressive responses to fluoxetine: A pilot study. Biological Psychiatry, 42, 546–552.
208) Cleare, A. J., & Bond, A. J. (2000). Ipsapirone challenge in aggressive men shows an inverse correlation between 5-HT1A receptor function and aggression. Psychopharmacology, 148, 344–349.
209) Umukoro, S., Aladeokin, A. C., & Eduviere, A. T. (2013). Aggressive behavior: A comprehensive review of its neurochemical mechanisms and management. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 18(2), 195–203. doi:10.1016/j.avb.2012.11.002
210) Blatchford, E., Bright, S., & Engel, L. (2021). Tripping over the other: Could psychedelics increase empathy?. Journal of Psychedelic Studies, 4(3), 163-170. doi:10.1556/2054.2020.00136
211) Lyons, T., & Carhart-Harris, R. L. (2018). Increased nature relatedness and decreased authoritarian political views after psilocybin for treatment-resistant depression. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 32(7), 811–819. doi:10.1177/0269881117748902
212) Breeksema, J. J., Kuin, B. W., Kamphuis, J., van den Brink, W., Vermetten, E., Schoevers, R. A. (2022). Adverse events in clinical treatments with serotonergic psychedelics and MDMA: A mixed-methods systematic review. Journal of Psychopharmacology. 36(10):1100-1117. doi:10.1177/0269881122111692
213) Esaki, H., Sasaki, Y., Nishitani, N., Kamada, H., Mukai, S., Ohshima, Y., Nakada, S., Ni, X., Deyama, S., Kaneda, K. (2023). Role of 5-HT1A receptors in the basolateral amygdala on 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine-induced prosocial effects in mice. Eur J Pharmacol. 946:175653. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2023.175653
214) Chakhssi, F., Kersten, T., de Ruiter, C., & Bernstein, D. P. (2014). Treating the untreatable: A single case study of a psychopathic inpatient treated with Schema Therapy. Psychotherapy, 51(3), 447–461. doi:10.1037/a0035773
215) Mohajerin, B., Howard, R. C. (2024). Effects of two treatments on interpersonal, affective, and lifestyle features of psychopathy and emotion dysregulation. Personal Ment Health. Feb;18(1):43-59. doi:10.1002/pmh.1593
216) Chester, D. (2023). Aggression as successful self‐control. Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 18. doi:10.1111/spc3.12832
217) Van Geel, M., Toprak, F., Goemans, A., Zwaanswijk, W., & Vedder, P. (2016). Are Youth Psychopathic Traits Related to Bullying? Meta-analyses on Callous-Unemotional Traits, Narcissism, and Impulsivity. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 48(5), 768–777. doi:10.1007/s10578-016-0701-0
218) Olivier, B. & Olivier. J. (2022). Aggression and Sexual Behavior: Overlapping or Distinct Roles of 5-HT1A and 5-HT1B Receptors. Serotonin and the CNS - New Developments in Pharmacology and Therapeutics. doi:10.5772/intechopen.104872
219) Verhoeven, W.M.A. & Tuinier, S. (2007). Serenics: Anti-aggression drugs throughout history. Clinical Neuropsychiatry. 4:135-143
220) Coccaro, E. F., Fanning, J. R., Phan, K. L., Lee, R. (2015). Serotonin and impulsive aggression. CNS Spectrums. 20: 295-302. doi:10.1017/S1092852915000310
221), 221), 221) Nelwan, M. L. (2018). Moderate the MAOA-L Allele Expression with CRISPR/Cas9 System. Preprints, 2018040275. doi:10.20944/preprints201804.0275.v3
222) Kosfeld, M., Heinrichs, M., Zak, P. J., Fischbacher, U., & Fehr, E. (2005). Oxytocin increases trust in humans. Nature, 435(7042), 673–676. doi:10.1038/nature03701
223) Marlin, B. J., Mitre, M., D’amour, J. A., Chao, M. V., & Froemke, R. C. (2015). Oxytocin enables maternal behaviour by balancing cortical inhibition. Nature, 520(7548), 499–504. doi:10.1038/nature14402
224) Carter, C. S., & Perkeybile, A. M. (2018). The Monogamy Paradox: What Do Love and Sex Have to Do With It? Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution, 6. doi:10.3389/fevo.2018.00202
225) Jørgensen, H., Riis, M., Knigge, U., Kjaer, A., & Warberg, J. (2003). Serotonin Receptors Involved in Vasopressin and Oxytocin Secretion. Journal of Neuroendocrinology, 15(3), 242–249. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2826.2003.00978.x
226) Tan, O., Martin, L. J., & Bowen, M. T. (2020). Divergent pathways mediate 5-HT1A receptor agonist effects on close social interaction, grooming and aggressive behaviour in mice: Exploring the involvement of the oxytocin and vasopressin systems. Journal of Psychopharmacology. doi:10.1177/0269881120913150
227) Studer, E., Näslund, J., Andersson, E., Nilsson, S., Westberg, L., et al. (2015). Serotonin Depletion-Induced Maladaptive Aggression Requires the Presence of Androgens. PLOS ONE 10(5): e0126462. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0126462
228) Crockett, M. J., Siegel, J. Z., Kurth-Nelson, Z., Ousdal, O. T., Story, G., Frieband, C., … Dolan, R. J. (2015). Dissociable Effects of Serotonin and Dopamine on the Valuation of Harm in Moral Decision Making. Current Biology, 25(14), 1852–1859. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2015.05.021
229) Seo, D., Patrick, C. J., & Kennealy, P. J. (2008). Role of serotonin and dopamine system interactions in the neurobiology of impulsive aggression and its comorbidity with other clinical disorders. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 13(5), 383–395. doi:10.1016/j.avb.2008.06.003
230) Eske, J. (2023). Dopamine and serotonin: Brain chemicals explained: https://www.medicalnewstoday.com/articles/326090
231) Reddy, K. J., Menon, K. R., Hunjan, U. G. (2018). Neurobiological Aspects of Violent and Criminal Behaviour: Deficits in Frontal Lobe Function and Neurotransmitters. International Journal of Criminal Justice Sciences: 13 pp. 44-54 (1). doi: 10.5281/zenodo.1403384
232) Manciu, F., Manciu, M., Ciubuc, J., Sundin, E., Ochoa, K., Eastman, M., … Bennet, K. (2018). Simultaneous Detection of Dopamine and Serotonin—A Comparative Experimental and Theoretical Study of Neurotransmitter Interactions. Biosensors, 9(1), 3. doi:10.3390/bios9010003
233) Hohmann, S., Becker, K., Fellinger, J., Banaschewski, T., Schmidt, M. H., Esser, G., & Laucht, M. (2009). Evidence for epistasis between the 5-HTTLPR and the dopamine D4 receptor polymorphisms in externalizing behavior among 15-year-olds. Journal of Neural Transmission, 116(12), 1621–1629. doi:10.1007/s00702-009-0290-1
234) Soderstrom, H. (2003). New evidence for an association between the CSF HVA:5-HIAA ratio and psychopathic traits. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 74(7), 918–921. doi:10.1136/jnnp.74.7.918
235) Mahadevia, D., Saha, R., Manganaro, A., et al. (2021). Dopamine promotes aggression in mice via ventral tegmental area to lateral septum projections. Nat Commun. 12(1):6796. doi:10.1038/s41467-021-27092-z
236) Chester, D. S., DeWall, C. N., Derefinko, K. J., Estus, S., Lynam, D. R., Peters, J. R., & Jiang, Y. (2015). Looking for reward in all the wrong places: dopamine receptor gene polymorphisms indirectly affect aggression through sensation-seeking. Social Neuroscience, 11(5), 487–494. doi:10.1080/17470919.2015.1119191
237) Johnston, J. E. (2017). Hooked on Homicide. Are serial killers addicted to murder.
238) Brogaard, B. (2018). Do All Serial Killers Have a Genetic Predisposition to Kill? Exploring a complex question.
239) Dully, H. F. L. (2021). Theoretical Analysis of the Biological Basis for Serial Killers' Cooling-Off Periods
240) Ramsland, K. (2006). Inside the Minds of Serial Killers: Why They Kill
241) Ogbonmide, T., Rathore, R., Rangrej, S. B., Hutchinson, S., Lewis, M., Ojilere, S., Carvalho, V., Kelly, I. (2023). Gene Therapy for Spinal Muscular Atrophy (SMA): A Review of Current Challenges and Safety Considerations for Onasemnogene Abeparvovec (Zolgensma). Cureus. 15(3):e36197. doi:10.7759/cureus.36197
242) Pearson, T. S. et al (2021). Gene therapy for aromatic L-amino acid decarboxylase deficiency by MR-guided direct delivery of AAV2-AADC to midbrain dopaminergic neurons: https://naked-science.ru/article/medicine/gene-therapy-for-aadc-deficiency
243) Mani, S., Jindal, D., Singh, M. (2022). Gene Therapy, A potential Therapeutic Tool for Neurological and Neuropsychiatric Disorders: Applications, Challenges and Future Prospective. Curr Gene Ther. doi:10.2174/1566523222666220328142427
244) Yoon, S. Y., Hunter, J. E., Chawla, S., Clarke, D. L., Molony, C., O’Donnell, P. A., … Wolfe, J. H. (2020). Global CNS correction in a large brain model of human alpha-mannosidosis by intravascular gene therapy. Brain, 143(7), 2058–2072. doi:10.1093/brain/awaa161
245) Alexander, B., Warner-Schmidt, J., Eriksson, T., Tamminga, C., … Kaplitt, M. G. (2010). Reversal of depressed behaviors in mice by p11 gene therapy in the nucleus accumbens. Science translational medicine, 2(54), 54ra76. doi:10.1126/scitranslmed.3001079
246) Kondaurova, E. M., Belokopytova, I. I., Kulikova, E. A., Khotskin, N. V., Ilchibaeva, T. V., Tsybko, A. S., Popova, N. K., Naumenko, V. S. (2023). On the role of serotonin 5-HT1A receptor in autistic-like behavior: сross talk of 5-HT and BDNF systems. Behav Brain Res. 438:114168. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2022.114168
247) Fido, D., Santo, M. G. E., Bloxsom, C. A. J., Gregson, M., & Sumich, A. L. (2017). Electrophysiological study of the violence inhibition mechanism in relation to callous-unemotional and aggressive traits. Personality and Individual Differences, 118, 44–49
248) Moore, T. M., Scarpa, A., & Raine, A. (2002). A meta-analysis of serotonin metabolite 5-HIAA and antisocial behavior. Aggressive Behavior, 28(4), 299–316. doi:10.1002/ab.90027
249) Xu, H., Xia, A., Wang, … Chen, M. (2020). An ultraportable and versatile point-of-care DNA testing platform. Science Advances, 6(17). doi:10.1126/sciadv.aaz7445
251) Gullapalli, A. R., Anderson, N. E., Yerramsetty, R., Harenski, C. L., & Kiehl, K. A. (2021). Quantifying the psychopathic stare: Automated assessment of head motion is related to antisocial traits in forensic interviews. Journal of Research in Personality, 92, 104093. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2021.104093
252) Sante-Costa, C. et al (2023). Psychopathic Traits Partly Explained the Reduced Vagal Tone of Batterers Coping with Acute Stress. Journal of Aggression Maltreatment & Trauma. doi:10.1080/10926771.2023.2294917
253) Dukart, J. et al (2021). Aberrant Brain Activity in Individuals With Psychopathy Links to Receptor Distribution, Gene Expression, and Behavior. Biological Psychiatry 91(12). doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2021.08.025
254) Gallup, A. et al (2021). People that score high on psychopathic traits are less likely to yawn contagiously. Sci Rep 11, 23779. doi:10.1038/s41598-021-03159-1
255) Torres, J. M., Sánchez, C., Ramı́rez, M. A., Morales, M., Bárcena, J., Ferrer, J., … Sánchez-Vizcaı́no, J. M. (2001). First field trial of a transmissible recombinant vaccine against myxomatosis and rabbit hemorrhagic disease. Vaccine, 19(31), 4536–4543. doi:10.1016/s0264-410x(01)00184-0
256) Craig, J. (2022). The controversial quest to make a 'contagious' vaccine. National Geographic: https://www.nationalgeographic.com/science/article/the-controversial-quest-to-make-a-contagious-vaccine
Last modified: 2024/04/17 21:05 by Volunto

All materials and concepts are free to distribute and use if this website (Antiviolence.io) is mentioned as the source (CC BY 4.0) CC Attribution 4.0 International
CC Attribution 4.0 International Donate Powered by PHP Valid HTML5 Valid CSS Driven by DokuWiki